Analysing effects of birth order on intelligence, educational attainment, big five and risk aversion in an Indonesian sample

Authored by journals.sagepub.com and submitted by mvea
image for Analysing effects of birth order on intelligence, educational attainment, big five and risk aversion in an Indonesian sample

Birth order effects in WEIRD countries have been investigated extensively, but little is known about whether similar patterns (i.e. effects on intelligence and educational attainment and lack of strong effects on Big Five) occur in the rest of the world. Because recent birth order research suggests that at least some of the confusion about the effects of birth order on the Big Five had to do with suboptimal methods (risk of overfitting owing to small samples, flexible model specification, and post hoc theorizing; see Rohrer, Egloff, & Schmukle, 2017 ), it seems wise to implement best practices to avoid these potential pitfalls. In our study, we aim to fill the research gap regarding birth order outside of the WEIRD world while adhering to best practices in birth order research. We ensured that all analyses were straightforward and comparable with recent work on WEIRD populations, applied appropriate control for sibship size, and conducted extensive robustness checks. On the basis of the assumption that previously reported birth order effects generalize, we predicted that intelligence, educational attainment, and intellect decrease with higher birth order while extraversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and risk aversion remain unaffected. Analyses were not preregistered; however, they are fully in line with earlier studies on the topic ( Rohrer et al., 2015 ), and extensive robustness checks are \provided.

The Republic of Indonesia—the world’s largest island country—is located in Southeast Asia. It is an interesting source for studying birth order effects not only because it is the world’s fourth most populous country and the most populous Muslim-majority country but also because it is home to a very diverse population that differs from the WEIRD samples on which theoretical accounts of birth order effects were based. In 2015, the estimated population was about 258 million people (median age: 28.4 years, 49.65% female), with a total fertility rate of 2.5 children per woman and a life expectancy of 68.6 years ( United Nations, 2015 ). In 2010, 87.18% of the total population was Muslim, 9.87% was Christian, 1.69% was Hindu, 0.72% was Buddhist, and 0.54% believed in another religion or did not believe in any religion ( Badan Pusat Statistik, 2010 ). According to the census, in 2010, there were over 300 ethnic groups in Indonesia. Of the total population, 40.22% was Javanese, 15.50% was Sundanese, and 44.28% belonged to one of many other ethnic groups (each less than 5%; Badan Pusat Statistik, 2010 ). Based on data from the UNESCO Institute of Statistics, the literacy rate in 2015 for people aged 15 years and older was 95.40%, and the mean number of years in school was 7.9 ( United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization, 2015 ).

These theories do not discuss potential influences by culture and instead seem to imply universal family dynamics. This suggests that birth order effects should not be specific to WEIRD populations; otherwise, one would have to conclude that family dynamics are less universal than assumed. In fact, explanations like resource dilution would predict even stronger linear birth order effects when families are large and resources are few. Non-WEIRD populations would therefore offer the most favourable conditions for detecting linear birth order effects due to resource dilution.

Prominent theoretical accounts of birth order effects like resource dilution ( Blake, 1981 ), the confluence model ( Zajonc & Markus, 1975 ), and the family niche model ( Sulloway, 1996 ) have taken no explicit stance on the potential for cultural specificity. The resource dilution theory focuses on the fact that with each additional child, parental resources are shared among more offspring. While the first-born child can enjoy undiluted parental resources until a sibling arrives, later-borns have to share from the very start—thus receiving less support for their intellectual development. This is thought to lead to a decrease in intelligence by birth order position ( Blake, 1981 ). The confluence model argues that earlier-born children grow up in a more stimulating intellectual environment than their younger siblings because first-borns interact mostly with adults in their early development phase, leading to a decrease in intelligence by birth order position ( Zajonc & Markus, 1975 ). The family niche model assumes that siblings compete for parental investment ( Trivers, 1985 ) and therefore develop strategies to increase parental attention by trying to fill different niches in one family ( Sulloway, 1996 ). The first-born takes the traditional niche resulting in higher values in neuroticism, conscientiousness, the intellectual aspects of openness, and the dominance aspect of extraversion. The second-born takes the rebellious niche, resulting in higher values in agreeableness, the sociability aspect of extraversion, and unconventional aspects of openness ( Sulloway, 2001 ).

Balinese names immediately reveal a person’s birth order: first-borns are called Wayan , second-borns Made , and so on. Given the everyday salience of sibling ranks, one might expect particularly pronounced birth order effects in Bali. However, previous birth order research on outcomes such as intelligence, educational attainment, and personality has almost exclusively focused on ‘WEIRD’ populations—populations from Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic countries ( Heinrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010 ). In helping to understand human universality and variability, researchers need to move the focus from WEIRD samples to more diverse populations ( Rad, Martingano, & Ginges, 2018 ).

To remain consistent with previous studies, and to avoid clumsy language, we will talk about birth order effects . However, it should be noted that the causal identification of birth order effects is a nontrivial issue that has not been addressed in the literature to date. While adjustment for family size can rule out certain obvious confounding backdoor paths (e.g. family socio-economic status may affect both personality and family size, which in turn affect the ratio of first-born to later-born children), other issues remain. For example, first-borns’ personality may affect their parents’ decision to have more children ( Jokela, 2010 ); personality may therefore actually causally affect birth order position. Such effects could result in birth order differences in personality, which do not reflect birth order effects but rather the effect of children’s personality on family size. Furthermore, parental age at birth and birth order position within-families are per definition confounded. Thus, birth order effects might be entangled with effects of parental age ( Arslan, Penke, Johnson, Iacono, & Mc Gue, 2014 ).

Following Grund, Ludtke, and Robitzsch (2017) , we performed multiple imputation for multilevel data on all individuals who were older than 14 and had data for at least two outcomes (usually years of education and intelligence tests in a previous wave). We included the identity of the mother as a grouping variable, a third-order polynomial for age, and the categorical interaction between sibling order and birth order. We let all variables that were correlated r > .05 predict each other (see Table S2 for a list of all variables included and https://laurabotzet.github.io/birth_order_ifls/3_ imputation.html for detailed code). To impute full birth order and sibling order from naive birth order and naive sibling order, we used the linear variables to regenerate the categorical interaction and ensured consistency (e.g. no sibling counts smaller than the birth order maximum). We used the R packages MICE package version 3.7.0 ( Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2010 ) and pan package version 1.6 ( Schafer, 1997 ) to impute the data. We generated 50 imputed datasets, ran all models 50 times, and aggregated estimates and standard errors using the mitml package version 0.3–7 ( Grund, Robitzsch, & Lüdtke, 2018 ). To evaluate the quality of imputations, we examined intraclass correlations, density plots, and trace plots. For further information, see https://laurabotzet.github.io/birth_order_ifls/3_imputation.html .

Like other large panel studies, the IFLS had systematic missing data. Some questions were only answered by a subset of participants (depending on age), some participants were absent for one of the waves, some mothers did not fill out the pregnancy questionnaire that we used to ascertain birth order, and some missing data were due to panel mortality. To impute birth order for individuals whose mothers had not filled out the pregnancy questionnaire, we computed variables, which we called ‘naive birth order’ and ‘naive sibling count’. This was simply the order according to birth year by people reporting the same mother. Pre-imputation analyses showed high agreement between the naive birth order and the full sibling order [ r = .91, 99.5% CI (.90, .91)], although systematic missing data are likely.

We reported the results of our model comparisons for each outcome based on full sibling order. Because of the number of outcomes and owing to calls for more stringent significance cut-offs in empirical science, we set the significance threshold to .005 ( Benjamin et al., 2017 ). We summarize results here briefly and report them in full online ( https://laurabotzet.github.io/birth_order_ifls/4_analyses.html ).

We performed additional robustness analyses based on maternal sibships and maternal pregnancy order (including stillbirths), and we tested the effect of excluding all individuals in sibships bigger than five. Furthermore, we repeated all analyses after multiple imputation (for details, see the following section, Handling missing data). We state where results changed depending on the analysis approach and report all robustness analyses on the website.

Birth order and sibship size were calculated for full sibships (same father and mother) based on the maternal reports of pregnancy and marriage history of all women aged 15–49 who participated in an IFLS wave. In all models, we adjusted for the categorical effect of sibship size (effects of sibship size 2, 3, 4, 5, and over 5), self-reported gender, a third-order polynomial for age, and a family random effect to account for dependencies within sibships. We then tested for a linear birth order effect, for non-linear effects (by testing categorical effects of birth order 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and over 5), and for potential interactions with number of siblings and went on to iteratively compare models to see whether each model improved upon the preceding one. We also reported estimates and confidence intervals (CIs) of the birth order effects to further examine the influence of birth order on the outcomes and compare effect sizes with the existing literature.

To further investigate the effects of birth order on additional outcomes, we included income in the last year, self-employment (0 = no, 1 = yes), smoking behaviour in the last year (0 = no, 1 = yes), category of work (six categories: casual worker in agriculture; casual worker not in agriculture; government worker; private worker; selfemployment; and unpaid family worker; 0 = no, 1 = yes), and sector of work (eight sectors: agriculture, forestry, fishing, and hunting; construction; electricity, gas, and water; finance, insurance, real estate, and business service; manufacturing; mining and quarrying; social services; and transportation, storage, and communication; 0 = no, 1 = yes). All of these outcomes were assessed in the fifth wave of the IFLS.

Risk aversion was assessed with an adaptive hypothetical lottery choice task for all respondents aged 15 or older. Analyses of the Mexican Family Life Survey had suggested that hypothetical lotteries yield similar results to lotteries that are paid out ( Hamoudi, 2006 ). Two different sets of questions, A and B, were asked (randomized order across participants). The sets differed in the amount of the payoffs and the variance of their expected payoffs. Set B’s certain payoffs were higher than Set A’s. The uncertain payoffs in Set B had higher coefficients of variation than did those in Set A, reflecting a higher risk–reward ratio (for a more detailed description, see Ng, 2013 ). Many participants gave inconsistent responses across the two tasks, and current research suggests that lottery tasks may be poor measures of individual differences in risk preferences compared with self-reports ( Frey, Pedroni, Mata, Rieskamp, & Hertwig, 2017 ).

Personality was assessed with the Big Five Index 15 (BFI 15) for all respondents aged 15 or older. The BFI 15 is based on the Big Five Inventory-SOEP ( Schupp & Gerlitz, 2008 ), which in turn is a short version of the Big Five Inventory and precludes examining single personality facets (e.g. intellect). All 15 items started with the phrase ‘I see myself as someone who …‘Three items were asked for each of the Big Five personality dimensions: extraversion (e.g. ‘… is talkative’), neuroticism (e.g. ‘… worries a lot’), conscientiousness (e.g. ‘… does a thorough job’), agreeableness (e.g. ‘… is considerate and kind to almost everyone’), and openness (e.g. ‘… is original, comes up with new ideas’). Participants expressed their agreement on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 = ‘disagree strongly’ to 5 = ‘agree strongly’). The BFI 15 is used in many large-scale surveys; in this case, the items were simply translated into Indonesian (details provided in Strauss et al., 2016 ). An earlier study by Wibowo, Yudiana, Reswara, and Jatmiko (2017) using the 44-item Big Five Inventory showed sufficient reliability (Cronbach’s alpha ranged between .69 and .85) but limited validity (exploratory factor analysis revealed eight instead of five factors) in an Indonesian sample.

Five intelligence subtests were conducted in the fifth wave of IFLS. All respondents aged 15 or older were asked to take part in these tests, which were as follows: (i) respondents answered a shortened version of a Raven’s matrices test that consisted of eight items. For each item, they were asked to identify the missing element out of six possible elements to complete a pattern. (ii) Respondents were asked to count backwards from 100 in steps of seven seconds. (iii) Respondents were given a delayed word recall test in which they heard a list of 10 nouns and had to recall as many words as possible four to five minutes later. (iv) Respondents were given an adaptive number series test in which they answered six out of 15 items. Each item showed a pattern of numbers with one missing value. Respondents had to name the missing number (e.g. ‘7–8–?–10’). The first three items were given to all participants. Based on the accuracy of the first three responses, a subsequent set of three items was chosen. A Rasch scoring model was used to identify a person’s ability for a given set of response patterns with varying difficulties; a composite score was calculated for each participant ( Strauss et al., 2016 ). (v) All respondents aged 15 to 59 participated in a math test. Each respondent answered five multiple-choice questions measuring mathematical abilities (three mathematical calculations and two math text problems).

We included intelligence, educational attainment, personality, and risk aversion as outcomes in our main analyses. Additional analyses (detailed results reported on the Supporting Information) included income, self-employment, working category (e.g. unpaid family worker), working sector, and smoking behaviour as outcomes. All outcomes are based on the fifth wave of the IFLS. Continuous outcomes were z -standardized ( M = 0, SD = 1) to make effect sizes easier to compare. For a detailed description of all outcomes, see Strauss et al. (2016) .

In keeping with earlier studies, we excluded families with multiple births or only children. The designs suitable for investigating birth order effects—comparison among children with the same number of siblings, or within-family comparisons—are not suitable for investigating whether only children differ from other children. In particular, when investigating only children, great care needs to be taken to control for systematic differences between families of different sizes. For analyses of birth order effects, we could only include individuals who participated in the fifth wave of the IFLS. A summary of the inclusion process for the full sibling birth order is shown in Table 1

In each wave, women aged 15 to 49 answered questions about their pregnancy and marriage history. These questions included information about the number and order of pregnancies as well as the gender and date of birth of each child. Overall, 15 983 women reported 49 868 pregnancies. Marriage history allowed us to approximately infer the identity of the father. Based on these data, we were able to construct full sibling order (based on the same mother and father) for 42 682 individuals.

Our data come from RAND’s Indonesian Family Life Survey (IFLS), an ongoing longitudinal study with 50 148 individuals living in Indonesia. Since 1993, five waves have been administered ( Frankenberg & Thomas, 2000 ; Strauss et al., 2000 ; Strauss, Witoelar, & Sikoki, 2016 ; Strauss, Witoelar, Sikoki, & Wattie, 2009 ). For the first wave, a sample of households that represented about 83% of the Indonesian population was approached. In the following waves, every household and all split-off households were contacted. All analyses reported in this study were run on data based on this representative national panel study. We therefore had no control over the exact sample size, but with N = 11 188, the sample size is comparable with or even larger than samples from recent literature on birth order effects.

Additional robustness analyses based on maternal sibship and maternal pregnancy order did not differ from the analyses reported here. For all details on the robustness analyses, see https://laurabotzet.github.io/birth_order_ifls/4_analyses_ robust.html. All analyses based on the imputed dataset yielded nonsignificant results (all p s .27). Contrary to our main analyses, we found no evidence for either a linear or a non-linear effect of birth order on educational attainment (linear compared with covariates-only model: p = .45, and categorical compared with linear model: p = .96). All details for the analyses based on the imputed dataset can be found online: https://laurabotzet.github.io/birth_order_ifls/4_analy-ses_imputed_data.html .

Additional analyses showed no significant effects of birth order on income (linear compared with covariates-only model: p = .51, categorical compared with linear model: p = .30, and interaction compared with categorical model: p = .56), self-employment (linear: p = .80, categorical: p = .86, and interaction: p = .62), and smoking behaviour (linear: p = .9996, categorical: p = .54, and interaction: p = .65). We found no birth order effects in the seven analyses on working category (linear: all p s > .26, categorical: all p s > .32, and interaction: all p s > .17) or in the eight analyses on working sector (linear: all p s > .14, categorical: all p s > .06, and interaction: all p s > .40). Sample sizes for some of these analyses might have been too small to detect birth order effects (income: n = 2477, self-employment: n = 3763, smoking behaviour: n = 6104, working category: n = 3763, and working sector: n = 3610). For more details on additional outcomes, see Table S3 and https:// laurabotzet.github.io/birth_order_ifls/4_analyses.html.

We found evidence for a non-linear effect of birth order on educational attainment [ X 2 (4, N = 6035) = 21.48, p .001]. A closer look at the categorical effect showed a checkmark-shaped pattern, indicating the same amount of educational attainment for first-borns compared with second-borns [estimation of effect: −0.06, 99.5% CI (−0.12, −0.005)], third-borns [0.02 (−0.06, 0.09)], and fourth-borns [0.09 (−0.01, 0.19)]. Fifth-borns had more educational attainment compared with first-borns [0.14 (0.01, 0.26)]. Differences between first-borns and sixth-borns and later-borns were not significant [0.08 (−0.05, 0.21)].

shows linear effects of birth order on all main outcomes, with effect sizes based on-standardized outcomes. The dotted line shows an estimate of the linear birth order effect on intelligence (= −0.14), and the grey area shows the 99.5% CI (−0.20, −0.07) based on a within-family analysis in a Western sample (sample reported in Rohrer et al., 2015 ; an additional within-family analysis was run to estimate the linear effect to ensure that the comparison was meaningful). Not only do the CIs of our estimates include zero, they also exclude the estimate for intelligence based on a large WEIRD sample.

For all main outcomes except for educational attainment, including birth order did not improve model fit, regardless of whether it was entered as a linear predictor, categorical predictor, or in combination with its interaction with sibship size (all p s > .02). Even though Akaike information criteria indicate model improvement in some analyses, differences in Akaike information criteria are miniscule, and comparisons f Bayesian information criteria as well as p -values indicate no model improvement. All results for model comparisons are summarized in Table 3

shows raw means, standard deviations, internal consistency, and a correlation matrix for age, gender, intelligence, educational attainment, extraversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and both risk aversion measurements based on our main sample with birth order information (= 11 188). Note that the internal consistency measures for short-form scales, as used here, probably underestimate reliability ( Eisenbarth, Lilienfeld, & Yarkoni, 2015 ). The correlation between age and intelligence [= −.10, 99.5% CI (−.13, −.06)] and age and the Big Five [extraversion:= .00 (−.03, .03), neuroticism:= −.13 (−.17, −.10), conscientiousness:= .24 (.20, .27), agreeableness:= .10 (.06, .13), and openness:= .00 (−.04, .04)] was consistent with trends found in WEIRD samples ( Nisbett et al., 2012 Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006 ). Being male was negatively correlated with extraversion [= −.13 (−.16, −.10)] and risk aversion [risk A:= −.13 (−.17, −.10); risk B:= −.12 (−.15, −.08)]. Intelligence was positively correlated with extraversion [= .06 (.02, .10)] and openness [= .08 (.05, .12)] and slightly negatively correlated with neuroticism [= −.05 (−.07, −.02)] and agreeableness [= −.04 (−.08, −.01)]. No consistent correlation pattern was visible for intelligence and risk aversion [risk A:= −.15 (−.18, −.11); risk B:= .05 (.01, .08)]. The correlations between the five dimensions of personality matched those found in a German sample ( Hahn, Gottschling, & Spinath, 2012 ) with positive correlations between all personality dimensions except for neuroticism. The two risk aversion tasks correlated moderately with each other [= .30 (.27, .34)].

Based on the results of the Raven’s matrices test, the math test, the backwards counting task, the delayed word recall, and the adaptive number series from all individuals who took part in wave 5 of the IFLS, we computed a g -factor of intelligence. Using a sample of participants who completed all five of the intelligence tests in wave 5 regardless of whether birth order information was available ( n = 27 526), we ran a confirmatory factor analysis expecting one factor. The g -factor explained 30% of variance on average in the five intelligence measurements (Raven’s matrices test: 42%, math test: 25%, backwards counting task: 20%, delayed word recall: 23%, and adaptive number series: 40%).

Our main sample of people for whom birth order could be computed differed systematically from those for whom the required information was missing. Our main sample was 25.31 years younger, and the percentage of females was 2 percentage points higher. The main sample was more intelligent ( d = 0.82) and had more years of educational attainment ( d = 0.82); it was also more extraverted ( d = 0.09), more neurotic ( d = 0.08), less conscientious ( d = −0.19), and less agreeable ( d = −0.13). It also scored higher on openness (d = 0.23) and showed differences in risk aversion with inconsistent signs across the two measures (decreased risk aversion for risk A: d = −0.10; increased for risk B: d = 0.08). These differences were all significant (all p s .001).

We found no birth order effects on intelligence, agreeableness, conscientiousness, extraversion, neuroticism, openness, or risk aversion, regardless of whether we included birth order as a continuous or categorical predictor, or whether we considered its interaction with sibship size. Model comparisons supported a small non-linear effect of birth order on educational attainment in the form of a checkmark-shaped pattern. However, this effect did not emerge when missing values were imputed.

Our results were consistent with null effects on agreeableness, conscientiousness, extraversion, neuroticism, and risk aversion found in WEIRD populations (Damian & Roberts, 2015; Lejarraga et al., 2019; Rohrer et al., 2015). Yet we found no effect of birth order on either intelligence or openness, in contrast to the small negative estimates reported for WEIRD populations (Barclay, 2015b; Damian & Roberts, 2015; Rohrer et al., 2015; Rohrer et al., 2017). In terms of educational attainment, our results were sensitive to the imputation of missing data; interpreting these results therefore requires caution. In WEIRD samples, higher birth order is related to lower educational attainment (Black et al., 2005; Booth & Kee, 2009; Härkönen, 2014; Kristensen & Bjerkedal, 2010), even in fully adopted sibling groups (Barclay, 2015b). However, in this Indonesian sample, higher birth order was related to higher educational attainment, if there was any association at all.

Our results are inconsistent with predictions from Blake’s (1981) resource dilution model, the confluence model (Zajonc & Markus, 1975), and Sulloway’s (1996) sibling roles. These theories made no allowance for fundamental cultural differences in family dynamics. Indonesia is poorer country than most previously studied countries, and its families are larger—Indonesian families, therefore, distribute fewer resources among more offspring. The resource dilution hypothesis would suggest that birth order effects, whatever their specific shape, are amplified in Indonesia; on this view, when resources are scarce, any additional investment leads to larger increases in returns, making any preferential allocation of resources more consequential. Observed effects fell short of initial predictions from theories on family dynamics that were based on the largest, best evidence available in the WEIRD world. It is therefore appropriate to look for other explanations. Perhaps birth order influences the social and parental expectations for first-born children in some countries, through remnants of Western cultural norms like primogeniture, or through policies such as parental leave (Barclay, 2015b). Children’s traits may then adapt due to external influences on their educational and occupational choices, such as a parent expecting a first-born to take over the family business (Barclay, Hällsten, & Myrskylä, 2017). This sort of indirect effect would be consistent with the small average birth order effects that are generally observed, as well as with the absence of those effects in a culturally different country.

It is important to note that our main sample of people for whom birth order could be computed differed systematically from those for whom the required information was missing. The large differences in intelligence and educational attainment are in part due to age differences. The main sample for whom birth order could be computed was 25 years younger than the rest of the sample, likely because birth order could only be computed for individuals whose mother took part in one of the waves of the IFLS, whereas outcome measurements were available for all participants. In the full sample, age correlated negatively with intelligence (r = −.39, p .001) and educational attainment (r = −.39, p .001); differences in intelligence and educational attainment might therefore actually reflect age differences. The main sample was 0.59 (0.58, 0.60) standard deviations more intelligent and attained 3.11 (3.06, 3.17) more years of educational attainment on average. Controlling for a third-order polynomial effect of age and a linear effect of our naive sibship size measurement reduced the adjusted mean differences to 0.33 (0.32, 0.33) standard deviations for intelligence and 1.94 (1.93, 1.96) years of educational attainment. Because the effects of interest for this study—namely, birth order effects—are within-family effects and because we controlled for potential age effects, the potential issues introduced by this discrepancy might not be grave. Nevertheless, generalizability of our findings might be limited to the younger, more intelligent, more educated Indonesian generation. The checkmark pattern that emerged for education should be interpreted with particular caution because it was not reproduced in the analyses of the multiple imputed datasets. This difference may reflect model error in the imputation or a lack of generalizability to the full sample.

In our data, we found no evidence for birth order effects on various outcomes related to type of employment and work sector, corroborating the emerging narrative that birth order is generally not an important predictor of life outcomes.

Limitations Because of limitations of the available data, there are several alternative explanations in favour of the existence of birth order effects that we could not rule out. First, the study population was limited. Our analyses of intelligence, educational attainment, Big Five, and risk aversion do not include individuals younger than 15 years—but according to Sulloway (2010), effects of birth order on personality should be especially visible during childhood and adolescence. Thus, we cannot rule out that we missed substantial birth order effects among younger Indonesians; we can only say that if they had existed, they had dissipated with age. Furthermore, the conclusions of our study are limited to present-day Indonesia and do not necessarily generalize to other (WEIRD or non-WEIRD) countries or across time. While the absence of birth order effects in present-day Indonesia casts doubt on broad theories that claim that such effects emerge universally, it does not rule out the possibility that birth order effects emerge under different societal conditions. Second, the outcome measures were limited. Intelligence was measured with a g-factor based on different subtests with no particular theoretical background and comparatively low reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = .61, average explained variance: 30%). Even though there is evidence that g-factor batteries correlate highly with each other despite measuring different mental abilities (Johnson, Bouchard, Krueger, McGue, & Gottesman, 2004), the reduced reliability might have impacted results. Given that we standardized the personality and intelligence outcomes, the limited reliability of our measurements probably attenuated the estimated effects. However, previous work on WEIRD samples was usually based on similarly short tests; our effect sizes are therefore comparable with the existing literature. The attenuation implies that our 99.5% CIs should be interpreted with care and considered in the context of the reliability of the outcome. In addition, the brief Big Five measure used made it impossible to test for effects on narrower facets. While the hunt for birth order effects on facets in WEIRD samples has not brought up consistent patterns, it is possible that such patterns exist in Indonesia. To take the critique concerning measurement one step further, one could argue that self-reports are generally not suitable for detecting birth order effects. This criticism applies to our analyses of the Big Five personality traits, but not necessarily to our measures of risk aversion and other reported outcomes (unless respondents systematically lied), and not to the assessment of intelligence. Sulloway (1999) suggested that first-borns respond in a more socially desirable manner to self-ratings than later-borns; this could cancel out existing birth order effects. This hypothesis could be tested using either other-reports of personality or behavioural outcomes that are not as easily affected by social desirability. It should be noted that there is little evidence for the social desirability hypothesis in WEIRD countries—older studies using personality comparisons made by other family members or even comparisons made by the targets themselves (e.g. ranking themselves among their siblings)—are hard to interpret as they might be affected by stereotypes. The only study we are aware of that uses other-reports by third parties does not provide much evidence for the expected effects (Jefferson, Herbst, & McCrae, 1998), and the few studies using alternative outcomes (i.e. behaviour in economic games, Courtiol, Raymond, & Faurie, 2009; Salmon, Cuthbertson, & Figueredo, 2016) suffer from various quality issues. However, recent population-based studies of choices of college major in Sweden found that earlier-borns were more likely to study engineering and medicine, while later-borns were more likely to study journalism, art, and business (Barclay et al., 2017). In addition, psychiatrist and blogger Scott Alexander (2018) reported a sizable overrepresentation of first-borns among his readership, which is heavily biased towards computer scientists. Such strong patterns regarding life choices in the absence of strong effects on intelligence and personality could perhaps be better explained through parental expectations, investment, and specific social norms. These major life choices may occasionally have hanger-on effects on the traits of intelligence and personality studied here. If so, the hanger-on effects would not be found across cultures (as would be expected if they were due to universal family dynamics); instead, they would be fairly specific to environments where university education is common and choices are affected by parents. An overall lack of good evidence is not the same as evidence for a lack of effects. Rohrer et al. (2017) stated that researchers who aim to venture down the path of analysing birth order effects on alternative outcomes like other-reports or behavioural measures should ensure that they follow best practices to avoid wrong conclusions. In particular, given the high effort involved in collecting behavioural data and observer reports for a sufficiently large sample to detect the potentially subtle effects of birth order, we encourage researchers to consider using a Registered Reports format lest their efforts result in a negative finding that might be hard to publish.

XROOR on March 8th, 2021 at 04:55 UTC »

In Korea tradition, when the parents get old, they live with the oldest son. There is intense pressure for him to be successful.

HegemonNYC on March 8th, 2021 at 04:39 UTC »

I’ve still got family in Vietnam, and birth order seems much more important there than in America. There is even a different name for eldest sibling, or eldest uncle, middle, youngest etc. Age difference continues into later generations too, like the children of the eldest uncle ‘outrank’ the children of the youngest, even if the children of the youngest are themselves older than the children of the eldest uncle. I think this is true in China as well

I suppose importance =!= personality, but it just seems to matter much more there.

[deleted] on March 7th, 2021 at 23:40 UTC »

No matter what, I will believe from my personal experience that younger sibilings get off easier.