Alcohol availability and neighborhood poverty and their relationship to binge drinking and related problems among drinkers in committed relationships

Authored by ncbi.nlm.nih.gov and submitted by orcie101

The authors examined the relationship of alcohol outlet density (AOD) and neighborhood poverty with binge drinking and alcohol-related problems among drinkers in married and cohabitating relationships and assessed whether these associations differed across sex. A U.S. national population couples survey was linked to U.S. Census data on AOD and neighborhood poverty. The 1,784 current drinkers in the survey reported on their binge drinking, alcohol-related problems, and other covariates. AOD was defined as the number of alcohol outlets per 10,000 persons and was obtained at the zip code level. Neighborhood poverty was as having a low (<20%) or high (≥20%) proportion of residents living in poverty at the census tract level. We used logistic regression for survey data to estimate odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals and tested for differences of associations by sex. Associations of neighborhood poverty with binge drinking were stronger for male than for female drinkers. The association of neighborhood poverty with alcohol-related problems was also stronger for men than for women. We observed no relationships between AOD and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems in this couples survey. Efforts to reduce binge drinking or alcohol-related problems among partners in committed relationships may have the greatest impact if targeted to male drinkers living in high-poverty neighborhoods. Binge drinking and alcohol-related problems, as well as residence in an impoverished neighborhood are risk factors for intimate partner violence (IPV) and other relationship conflicts.

We used a national population-based survey of couples with detailed individual-level alcohol information and neighborhood information on alcohol availability and poverty to address our questions of interest. (a) Is alcohol availability and neighborhood poverty associated with binge drinking and alcohol-related problems among drinkers in committed relationships? (b) Do these associations vary by sex? We hypothesized that a high AOD and neighborhood impoverishment would be positively associated with binge drinking and alcohol-related problems and that sex would modify the associations between neighborhood characteristics and drinking outcomes. Our analysis of sex differences was less prescribed due to the limited research in this area.

Alcohol availability and neighborhood impoverishment may influence drinking patterns and alcohol-related violence differently for men and women. Both Cunradi (2010) and Karriker-Jaffe (2011) have called for further research on gender differences in the effects of neighborhood disadvantage and AOD on alcohol consumption and IPV. Some studies show a stronger association between neighborhood disadvantage and alcohol and mental health outcomes for men as compared with women ( Karvonen & Rimpelä, 1997 ; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2003 ; Simons, Johnson, Beaman, Conger, & Whitbeck, 1996 ), although the evidence for this link remains tentative. For this study, we reasoned that men of lower socioeconomic status who tend to live in impoverished neighborhoods (which tend to have a higher AOD), may binge drink more frequently than their male counterparts in more affluent neighborhoods ( National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2006 ). Greater frequency of binge drinking among these men may be related to increased alcohol availability, cultural, social, or stress-related reasons. In contrast, women of lower socioeconomic status who tend to live in impoverished areas may binge drink less than their more affluent female counterparts due to more conservative cultural or social norms or greater caregiver responsibilities.

Higher rates of alcohol-related violence in areas with high AOD and in impoverished neighborhoods may be related to the characteristics and drinking patterns of residents. For example, impoverished neighborhoods are disproportionately composed of individuals with relatively low levels of education and income and, though these individuals tend to have a lower average volume of alcohol intake, they may binge drink with greater frequency than their better educated and wealthier counterparts ( National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2006 ). Although studies have examined the relationship of AOD and neighborhood poverty to measures of alcohol intake such as average volume, heavy drinking, or frequency of drinking, we know of none that has evaluated binge drinking (typically defined as ≥5 or ≥4 drinks per occasion for males and females). Binge drinking may lead to acute intoxication, impaired decision making or increased aggressive behavior ( Peterson, Rothfleisch, Zelazo, & Pihl, 1990 ; Sayette, Wilson, & Elias, 1993 ). Caetano, Schafer, and Cunradi (2001) reported higher rates of male-to-female partner violence for males who are frequent binge drinkers, that is, consuming five or more drinks on occasion at least once a week. General population surveys correlate binge drinking with both being in a fight and being the target of aggression while drinking ( Rehm et al., 2003 ). This type of drinking may be the pattern of drinking most likely to elevate the risk of violence.

One explanation for the link between AOD and alcohol-related harms, including IPV, is that the greater physical availability of alcohol increases individual-level alcohol consumption thereby elevating the risk of experiencing such harms ( Speer, Gorman, Labouvie, & Ontkush, 1998 ). Heavy drinkers may also cluster in neighborhoods with higher alcohol availability ( Scribner, Cohen, & Fisher, 2000 ). For impoverished neighborhoods, resident alcohol consumption may be linked to several factors, including increased psychological distress, diminished social ties and shared norms between neighbors, and the availability of fewer institutional resources to address residents’ needs ( Hill & Angel, 2005 ; Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002 ). Although relationships between AOD or neighborhood poverty and increased risk for partner violence have been identified (as well as between alcohol consumption and IPV), the link between these neighborhood characteristics and drinking outcomes in couples is less clear. In this study, we sought to test the relationships for AOD and neighborhood poverty to alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems for drinkers in married or cohabiting relationships. This is a preliminary study to examine drinking and related problems in couples as a putative mechanism linking neighborhood characteristics to IPV. To our knowledge, this is the first study to test these relationships using couples data.

Because studies have reported that impoverished neighborhoods tend to have high AOD and both of these factors may be associated with alcohol intake, in our adjusted analyses we accounted for neighborhood poverty when examining the relation between AOD and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems and accounted for AOD when examining the relation between neighborhood poverty and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems. To isolate the relationship between AOD and binge drinking (a specific type of drinking), we adjusted for the average number of alcoholic drinks per week consumed. Since we anticipated a high AOD may lead to greater number of drinks per week and a high frequency of binge drinking, both of which could increase the risk of alcohol-related problems, we did not adjust for number of drinks per week in our adjusted analyses of alcohol-related problems. For associations related to neighborhood poverty, we did not control for other neighborhood census level socioeconomic factors since many were strong correlates of neighborhood poverty (e.g., percentage unemployed).

In each model, we initially adjusted for all sociodemographic factors and certain behavioral factors listed and as categorized in , including illicit drug use, IPV, and child physical abuse identified a priori as potential confounders. Those potential confounders found not to change estimates to a meaningful degree when removed from our initial models, generally by less than 10%, were dropped from the models ( Maldonado & Greenland, 1993 ; Rothman, 2002 ). Adjustment variables for each model are noted in .

We calculated descriptive statistics for individual and neighborhood characteristics across categories of binge drinking and alcohol-related problems. We estimated unadjusted and adjusted associations for AOD or neighborhood poverty with binge drinking or alcohol-related problems. We used standard logistic regression models for survey data to estimate odds ratios (OR) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CI) of associations for alcohol-related problems; similar multinomial logistic regression methods were used to estimate associations related to our three-category measure of binge drinking. To evaluate the moderating effect of male and female sex on the associations between AOD or neighborhood poverty and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems, we fit logistic regression models with the multiplicative interaction terms for sex and AOD and sex and neighborhood poverty. We used a survey-based Wald test to derive a p value for the interaction terms.

All statistical analyses used the survey sampling adjustment implemented in Stata 10.0 and were weighted to account for the complex sampling design. More specifically, because our adjustment for clustering at the primary sampling unit level also accounts for clustering below this level, no further adjustment for clustering in zip codes, census tracts, or couples was necessary ( Binder, 1983 ; Williams, 2000 ). We specified the subpopulation of interest (i.e., drinkers) in our analysis to ensure appropriate estimation of standard errors ( Cochran, 1977 ; Rao, 2003 ).

These sociodemographic and behavioral characteristics for survey respondents represent potential confounders in assessing the relationships between neighborhood characteristics (i.e., AOD and neighborhood poverty) and drinking outcomes (i.e., binge drinking and alcohol-related problems). We defined potential confounders as factors plausibly related to both AOD or neighborhood poverty and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems but not in the causal pathway ( Koepsell & Weiss, 2003 ) based on our knowledge of the literature and the relationships between factors. For example, an individual’s income was believed plausibly associated with neighborhood poverty and frequency of binge drinking and, therefore, included as a potential confounder.

Survey participants who reported any use of cocaine, heroin, or marijuana in the 12 months prior to the survey were categorized as having a history of illicit drug use; otherwise participants were considered not to have used illicit drugs. To assess IPV, respondents were asked about a series of physically violent behaviors from the Conflict Tactics Scale, Form R, a validated and commonly used scale for assessing IPV ( Straus, 1990 ). These violent behaviors included threw something; pushed, grabbed, or shoved; slapped; kicked, bit, or hit; hit or tried to hit with something; beat up; choked; burned or scalded; forced to have sex; threatened with a knife or gun; and had knife or gun used against you/partner. Each respondent reported separately their behavior toward their partner and their partner’s behavior toward them. IPV was positive for a respondent if they or their partner reported that either partner had committed any of the specified violent behaviors in the past year; otherwise respondents were categorized as not having experienced IPV. Respondents who reported a parent or caregiver had ever hit them with something; beaten them up; burned or scalded them; threatened them with a knife or gun; or used a knife or gun against them during childhood were categorized as having a history of childhood physical abuse, otherwise respondents were categorized as not having experienced childhood physical abuse.

Ethnicity was self-reported and categorized as follows: those reporting Hispanic ethnicity were classified as Hispanic. The remaining respondents were classified as either non-Hispanic Black (“Black”), non-Hispanic White (“White”), or non-Hispanic Other (“Other”). Total household income was collected from one respondent in each couple to include all sources of income in the household; both respondents in each couple have the same household income. We categorized total household income into (a) <US$10,000, (b) US$10,000 to US$19,999, (c) US$20,000 to US$29,999, (d) US$30,000 to US$39,999, and (e) ≥US$40,000. Other characteristics considered included sex (male and female), age (18–29, 30–39, 40–49, and ≥50 years old), educational attainment (<high school, high school degree or equivalent, and >high school) and employment status (4 categories; employed, unemployed, retired, and homemaker/other).

Participants who responded affirmatively to having experienced ≥1 of 25 alcohol-related social or dependence problems in the past year were classified as having an alcohol-related problem ( Cunradi et al., 1999 ); otherwise respondents were classified as not having an alcohol-related problem. Each of the 25 questions specifically implicated alcohol as being a contributing factor to the social or dependence problem. Social consequences covered problems with police, accidents, health-related problems, work-related problems, relationship problems, arguments or fights, and financial problems. Alcohol dependence symptoms were associated with a physical addiction to alcohol, including withdrawal, tolerance, and impaired control. Other alcohol-related symptoms, experiences, and feelings were assessed (e.g., in the last year: “I have often taken a drink the first thing when I wake up in the morning”; “My drinking interfered with my spare time activities or hobbies”; and “Sometimes I needed a drink so badly that I couldn’t think of anything else”). We combined alcohol-related dependence and social problems into one measure because in this general population sample of couples alcohol-related problems were relatively rare and because this was an initial assessment of the hypothesis that sex modifies the association of AOD or neighborhood poverty and alcohol-related problems.

Binge drinking was defined as consuming ≥5 drinks in a single day. To measure binge drinking, we asked respondents how frequently they consumed 5 to 7, 8 to 11, and 12 or more drinks of any kind of alcoholic beverage in a single day during the last 12 months (nine categories that ranged from every day or nearly every day to one time/past year to never). Responses were converted into an average monthly binge drinking frequency, and we classified individuals into three groups: (a) those who did not binge drink, (b) those who binge drank, on average, >0–3, and (c) ≥4 times per month in the past year. Because we hypothesized that those who binge drink most frequently would be mostly influenced by alcohol availability, we selected a three-level category of binge drinking to assess associations related to frequent binge drinking rather than the more commonly used dichotomous measure.

Weekly drinking volume combined two self-reported drinking variables (i.e., frequency and quantity). Respondents were asked (a) how frequently they usually drank wine, beer, or liquor (11 categories that ranged from ≥3 times/day to <1 time/year to never) and (b) what proportion of drinking occasions (five categories that ranged from nearly every time to never) they drank 1 to 2, 3 to 4, and 5 to 6 glasses of each beverage type. On the basis of these reports, we estimated the average number of drinks per week consumed by respondents in the past 12 months. One drink was the equivalent to 12 g of absolute alcohol or a 4-oz glass of wine, 1-oz shot of distilled spirit, or 12-oz can or bottle of beer.

We used the 1990 U.S. Census measure of the percentage of residents living in poverty to assess neighborhood impoverishment at the census tract level. This measure is robust and valid, similar to multifactor composite measures for detecting socioeconomic gradients across health conditions ( Krieger et al., 2002 , 2003a , 2003b ). We dichotomized the percentage living in poverty according to the U.S. Census definition of high-poverty areas ( U.S. Census Bureau, 1995 ). Residents of areas with <20% living in poverty were classified as living in an “affluent” area whereas residents of areas with ≥20% living in poverty were classified as living in an “impoverished” area. The poverty level for individuals and families used by the U.S. Census are based on pretax income and are updated every year to reflect the Consumer Price Index. In 1990, average poverty thresholds varied from US$6,652 for a person living alone to US$26,848 for a family of nine or more members ( U.S. Census Bureau, 1991 ). Because census tracts may cross over zip code boundaries, census tracts were spatially assigned to corresponding zip codes in which the majority of the census tract was physically located.

North American Industry Classification System (NAICS; U.S. Census Bureau, 2001 ) codes were used to identify the number and types of alcohol outlets by zip code. We obtained NAICS code data from the 1997 Economic Census. The Economic Census is conducted every 5 years, in years ending in “2” and “7”; we selected 1997 data to best replicate the accrual period for the couples survey data. We incorporated state-level retail licensing law information because alcohol sales at some types of vendors such as drug or grocery stores (denoted in the NAICS codes) vary depending on state and local laws ( Distilled Spirits Council of the United States [DISCUS], 1996 ). We obtained state-level retail licensing law information from DISCUS (1996) to determine which states allowed alcohol sales for various business types (e.g., drug stores or grocery stores). Counts of these business types were included in aggregate counts for zip codes in states that allowed alcohol sales in these types of venues. We further classified alcohol outlet types into off- and on-premise AOD. Off-premise outlets, such as convenience stores, sell alcohol for consumption elsewhere; on-premise outlets include bars and restaurants where alcohol is consumed on-site. Our estimates of alcohol outlets were generally consistent with those in the Adams Liquor Handbook, a comprehensive alcohol sourcebook ( Adams Business Media, 1999 ). We calculated off-premise, on-premise, and total AOD as the number of outlets per 10,000 persons (based on 1990 U.S. Census population estimates). Because some establishments sold alcohol both off- and on-premise, measures add to more than the total in some zip codes.

This study is based on the 1,784 survey participants classified as current drinkers (weighted count; n = 1,949). Current drinkers consumed ≥1 alcoholic drink in the 12 months before the survey. Because our question of interest was related to a specific type of drinking (i.e., binge drinking) and having an alcohol-related problem, and since only current drinkers could report binge drinking or having an alcohol-related problem, the 1,407 nondrinkers were excluded; 39 individuals were excluded because of missing data. Our study examined AOD at the zip code level (n = 497), the smallest geographic area for which alcohol outlet data were available ( U.S. Department of Commerce Economics and Statistics Administration, 1997 ). Neighborhood impoverishment was from the 1990 U.S. Census at the tract level (n = 513; GeoLytics, 1998 ). We used census tract boundaries from the 1990 instead of the 2000 Census because they provided a closer alignment to the sampling units used for this couples’ survey.

Individual-level data come from a national U.S. survey of couples using a multistage random probability sample of individuals 18 years of age or older from the 48 contiguous states in 1995 ( Cunradi, Caetano, Clark, & Schafer, 1999 ). In the original survey, 100 primary sampling units composed of standard statistical metropolitan areas, counties, or groups of counties were selected. Within each primary sampling unit, 15 secondary sampling units made up of combinations of census blocks were selected. Enumerators then listed all housing units in the secondary sampling units, and housing units were selected at random. For each selected house with one adult ≥18 years of age, an adult was selected at random and interviewed. When this person was married or live with someone in a romantic relationship for greater than 6 months, the partner was then also interviewed. In the end, 1,925 married or cohabitating participants were eligible, and 1,615 respondents and their partners (3,230 individuals) across 37 states and 588 zip codes completed a private face-to-face structured interview in English or Spanish. The overall response rate was 85%.

Associations of total AOD were similar to estimates of off- and on-premise AOD (data not shown). In post hoc unadjusted analysis of a binary measure of AOD, residents of high AOD areas (≥5 per 10,000 residents) compared with residents of low AOD areas (<5 per 10,000 residents) had decreased odds of binge drinking >0–3 times per month, OR = 0.7, 95% CI [0.5, 1.1], and increased odds of binge drinking ≥4 times per month, OR = 2.2, 95% CI [1.0, 4.6], compared with nonbinge drinkers (overall p value for binge drinking = .01). In adjusted analysis of the binary measure of AOD, we observed similar point estimates, residents of high AOD areas versus residents of low AOD areas were 0.8 times as likely to binge drink >0–3 times per month, 95% CI [0.5, 1.2], but were 1.5 times as likely to binge drink ≥4 times per month, 95% CI [0.6, 3.5], compared with nonbinge drinkers (overall p value for binge drinking = .36). However, the wide confidence intervals which include 1.0 (no association) in the adjusted analysis preclude us from being able to make any conclusions concerning these findings. No association was observed in unadjusted and adjusted analysis between the binary measure of AOD and alcohol-related problems (data not shown).

In contrast, the relationship between neighborhood poverty and binge drinking differed for men and women (p values for multiplicative interaction ≤ .01 in unadjusted and adjusted analysis; see ). In adjusted analysis, men living in impoverished neighborhoods had more than three times the odds of binge drinking compared with men living in more affluent neighborhoods. In contrast, women living in impoverished neighborhoods were at inverse adjusted odds of binge drinking relative to their female counterparts in more affluent neighborhoods, though CIs were compatible with a wide range of estimates including no association. Similar findings were observed for the relationship between neighborhood poverty and alcohol-related problems; the adjusted and unadjusted associations differed for men and women (p values for multiplicative interaction ≤ .01). Men living in impoverished neighborhoods were more apt to have alcohol-related problems than their male counterparts in more affluent neighborhoods whereas women in impoverished areas were less apt to have alcohol-related problems relative to their female counterparts living in more affluent neighborhoods, though estimates among women had confidence intervals compatible with no association.

In both unadjusted and adjusted analysis, AOD did not appear associated with binge drinking (see ). In unadjusted analysis, a one-unit increase in AOD was associated with no apparent increase in the odds of binge drinking >0–3 or ≥4 times per month compared with respondents who did not binge drink. Similar results were evident in our adjusted analysis. The association between AOD and binge drinking in adjusted analyses did not differ for men and women (p value for multiplicative interaction = .21). Similar to binge drinking, AOD appeared unassociated with alcohol-related problems in unadjusted or adjusted analysis and did not vary across sex (p value for multiplicative interaction = .29). All adjusted estimates for AOD accounted for sex, age, race/ethnicity, income, employment status, and neighborhood poverty. For frequency of binge drinking, we also adjusted for number of alcoholic drinks per week.

Mean total AOD was lowest among those who binge drank ≥4 times per month in the past year; mean off-premise AOD did not appear to vary across frequency of binge drinking but mean on-premise AOD was slightly lower among those who binge drank ≥4 times per month relative to the other groups (See ). More striking differences were observed for neighborhood poverty. Nearly one quarter of those who binge drank ≥4 times per month lived in areas with ≥20% of residents living in poverty compared with <10% of nonbinge drinkers. As expected, nonbinge drinkers had a lower average weekly number of drinks than those who binge drank ≥4 times per month. More women than men abstained from binge drinking and five times more men than women binge drank ≥4 times per month. Relative to nonbinge drinkers, binge drinkers of ≥4 times per month were disproportionately Black or Hispanic; had less than a high school degree; were unemployed; had an annual household income <US$20,000; or reported illicit drug use, IPV, or child abuse. Comparable trends were observed for those with and without alcohol-related problems ( ). All variables had <2% missing data except household income which had 5.5%.

We examined one mechanism linking neighborhood conditions and the occurrence of IPV in couples, hypothesizing that neighborhood poverty and greater alcohol outlet density would increase binge drinking and alcohol-related problems for drinkers in couples. We found no association between AOD and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems. However, neighborhood poverty was positively associated with both binge drinking and alcohol-related problems among male partners who drink. Neighborhood poverty was inversely associated with binge drinking and alcohol-related problems among female partners who drink, though confidence intervals were compatible with a wide range of estimates including the possibility of no association. This is the first study to examine and find that the association between neighborhood poverty and the drinking outcomes of binge drinking and alcohol-related problems may differ for men and women drinkers.

Neighborhood poverty was positively associated with binge drinking and alcohol-related problems among male drinkers and is consistent with alcohol-related violence like physical assaults that tend to be concentrated among men (U.S. Department of Justice, 2008). In couples, male binge drinking and alcohol-related problems are associated with increased rates for male perpetrated partner violence (Caetano et al., 2001). Male drinkers living in impoverished neighborhoods may binge drink more often than men in more affluent areas as a palliative escape from a stressful living environment. For example, Jones-Webb et al. (1997) reported that impoverished neighborhoods, associated with more alcohol-related problems for Black men, were characterized by low family incomes, more unemployment, and high population densities. Alternatively, it could be that a low level of collective efficacy (Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997) inhibits residents’ ability to constrain this type of high-risk drinking. Other factors such as the types and characteristics of drinking places (e.g., cheap drinks in bars that offer minimal food vs. restaurants with more expensive drinks) may differ in wealthy and impoverished neighborhoods and may also influence binge drinking (LaVeist & Wallace, 2000). These same neighborhood characteristics may similarly increase male drinker’s risk of alcohol-related social or dependence problems.

In contrast, women living in impoverished neighborhoods may have different caretaking responsibilities and may ascribe to cultural norms that make the frequency of binge drinking lower than that found among female counterparts in wealthier areas. For example, compared with nonpoverty areas, poverty areas have a higher prevalence of larger families (1 in 25 vs. 1 in 75) and families not maintained by a married couple (35% vs. 17%; U.S. Census Bureau, 1995); both family types may be more likely to have increased caretaking responsibilities for females. Other norms and attitudes governing alcohol use as well as coping mechanisms for stress-related poverty may also differ for men and women. There is greater stigma associated with drinking for women than men (Herd, 1997). Greenfield and Room (Greenfield & Room, 1997) reported that women compared with men are less accepting of drinking and drunkenness (a characteristic related with binge drinking). It may be that women, regardless of neighborhood poverty have similar norms and attitudes about drinking. This could explain the similar associations between neighborhood impoverishment and alcohol outlets for women in poverty and nonpoverty neighborhoods. Alternatively, drinking and depression could represent different, but parallel ways of reacting to stressors for men and women, respectively (Thoits, 1995).

Our finding that AOD was not associated with binge drinking or alcohol-related problems among drinkers is somewhat surprising. Although studies of AOD and individual-level alcohol intake have been mixed (Gruenewald et al., 2002; Pollack et al., 2005; Scribner et al., 2000), many ecological studies have found positive associations between AOD and alcohol-related harms, for example, sexually transmitted diseases, motor vehicle accidents, and homicide and other violence (Cohen et al., 2006; Scribner et al., 1999; Treno et al., 2007; Zhu et al., 2004) and alcohol sales (Gruenewald, Ponicki, & Holder, 1993), which suggest that greater AOD increases individuals’ alcohol intake in that vicinity. In post hoc analysis, our binary measure of AOD showed a strong, positive association with binge drinking. However, this secondary analysis should be interpreted cautiously given it was exploratory, not hypothesized a priori and therefore may be more likely to have occurred by chance. This finding may nonetheless guide future researchers to consider whether a threshold or other measure of AOD may be a better parameterization for estimating associations between AOD and individual-level drinking measures.

Furthermore, ecological studies examining AOD and alcohol-related harms generally do not have information about the drinking status of individuals at the time the event occurred. It may be that those who experienced the harm were not drinking or may not have alcohol-related problems when it took place. Thus, there may be no association between AOD and individuals’ alcohol intake and the observed relationship with AOD may be related to other factors. For example, alternative mechanisms plausibly linking greater AOD and IPV include: (a) the loosening of normative controls against violence in disorganized neighborhoods where the number of alcohol outlets are often greater and (b) the clustering of high-risk couples in such disorganized neighborhoods (Cunradi, 2010).Furthermore, given that AOD is positively correlated with neighborhood poverty (Pollack et al., 2005), by adjusting for neighborhood poverty we may have accounted for the influence of AOD on binge drinking and alcohol-related problems. Then again, census tracts were not geographically nested within zip code boundaries, and we may have been unable to completely account for census tract–level poverty when estimating associations for AOD (and vice versa). Most studies of AOD and alcohol-related harms also do not account for individual-level sociodemographic and behavioral factors. That we were able to account for such factors could be responsible for our seemingly contradictory results. This explanation seems unlikely given that estimates for AOD were similar whether we adjusted for individual-level factors and that unadjusted estimates showed no association between AOD and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems.

Limitations of the measure of AOD, which was only available at the zip code level, may have precluded us from being able to identify associations. Our census tract–level measure of neighborhood poverty may have been a more valid and robust measure than AOD and as such, better able to detect variations in binge drinking and alcohol-related problems. Zip code–level measures of neighborhood characteristics appear less sensitive to variations in health outcomes than smaller units such as census tracts or blocks (Krieger et al., 2003a). However, in a related article (McKinney et al., 2009) based on this same survey data set we found that AOD was positively associated with IPV. We did not have the data to examine AOD using other types of measures such as number of roadway miles; others who have examined AOD using both geographic and population density measures have found that these measures yield similar findings (Scribner et al., 1999). We were also unable to analyze AOD according to specific types of alcohol outlets that may be particularly related to the examined drinking outcomes. In some studies, bars contribute disproportionately to alcohol-related social problems (Gruenewald et al., 2002; Gruenewald, Freisthler, Remer, Lascala, & Treno, 2006; Gruenewald & Remer, 2006; LaVeist & Wallace, 2000; Pollack et al., 2005). Similarly, convenience stores typically sell cold, ready-to-drink alcoholic beverages for quick consumption in large quantities (LaVeist & Wallace, 2000) and have long hours of operation. These settings may confer greater risks for binge drinking or alcohol-related problems than other types of alcohol outlets.

That is, the association between AOD and binge drinking or alcohol-related problems may exist but our inability to measure it accurately could have precluded us from observing the association. Our measure of AOD was likely subject to nondifferential misclassification since alcohol outlet data on sole proprietors and family-owned businesses were not collected by the U.S. Economic Census and we were unable to incorporate detailed alcohol-licensing laws for counties or cities into our estimates of AOD. Our measure of AOD was based on 1997 data (and poverty was based on 1990 data) and may not correspond with the environment in 1995, the time individual-level respondent data was collected and may be another source of nondifferential misclassification. In addition, where some respondents procured their alcohol may not be in the zip code in which an individual lives. These sources of misclassification may have biased our estimates.

Our findings can be generalized to drinkers who are married or cohabiting, but may not be representative of drinkers not living with a partner who may be younger and more likely to binge drink (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2006). Data from 1995 may not be reflective of the current situation. For example our survey collected information on binge drinking defined as ≥5 drinks for both men and women, but binge drinking was redefined in 2004 as ≥4 drinks among women and ≥5 among men (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2004). As fewer women would report binge drinking at ≥5 drinks than ≥4 drinks, more women would have been classified in the lower binge drinking categories (none or ≥0–3); this would bias our estimates for women drinkers toward the null. Our categorization of binge drinking was intended to capture associations of AOD and neighborhood poverty related to high-frequency binge drinking. However, our measure may not have captured certain characteristics such as the duration of this binge drinking (e.g., for how long a respondent had been binge drinking ≥4 times per month) or the peak number of drinks per binge drinking occasion. Thus, we may have not captured drinking patterns most likely associated with AOD. This notion is consistent with the “prevention paradox” which notes that drinking problems commonly occur during drinking that is generally typical (Rossow & Romelsjo, 2006; Spurling & Vinson, 2005).

In conclusion, these findings provide new information about associations of AOD and neighborhood poverty with binge drinking and alcohol-related problems in a population of married and cohabitating drinkers. Our findings suggest that male drinkers who live in high-poverty areas are at increased odds of binge drinking and having alcohol-related problems relative to male drinkers living in more affluent areas. Efforts to reduce these alcohol outcomes may have the most impact if targeted to this subpopulation. To build on this study, future research could use other analytical approaches such as structural equation modeling to detail the interrelationships between AOD, neighborhood poverty, and drinking outcomes (i.e., binge drinking and alcohol-related problems) in predicting IPV, and formally test binge drinking and alcohol-related problems as mediating variables. Our analysis used an epidemiologic approach; a geospatial approach may offer further insight. In addition, a more detailed measure of AOD at a smaller level such as census tract and being able to delineate specific types of outlets could help clarify the relationship between AOD and binge drinking and alcohol-related problems.

hammerertv on March 28th, 2018 at 01:52 UTC »

Yeah for us blokes it's just cocaine right?

dj2short on March 28th, 2018 at 00:59 UTC »

I immediately pictured Queen Cersei drinking wine lol

diegojones4 on March 28th, 2018 at 00:15 UTC »

Free time and boredom.