Church attendance, allostatic load and mortality in middle aged adults

Authored by journals.plos.org and submitted by mvea

Churchgoers (at least once a year) comprised 64.0% of the study cohort (n = 3782). Non-churchgoers had significantly higher overall mean AL scores and higher prevalence of high-risk values for 3 of the 10 markers of AL than did churchgoers. In bivariate analyses non-churchgoers, compared to churchgoers, had higher odds of an AL score 2–3 (OR 1.24; 95% CI 1.01, 1.50) or ≥4 (OR 1.38; 95% CI 1.11, 1.71) compared to AL score of 0–1. More frequent churchgoers (more than once a week) had a 55% reduction of all-cause mortality risk compared with non-churchgoers. (HR 0.45, CI 0.24–0.85) in the fully adjusted model that included AL.

Data for this study are from NHANES III (1988–1994). The analytic sample (n = 5449) was restricted to adult participants, who were between 40–65 years of age at the time of interview, had values for at least 9 out of 10 clinical/biologic markers used to derive AL, and had complete information on church attendance.

Funding: This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health grants UL1TR000445 (MB), 1K01HL88735 (MB), 1R25HL126145 (BB), P20MD000182 (KN, DM), UL1TR000124 (KN, AB), P30AG021684 (KN, KD), P60MD002249 (MS), U54MD008176 (MS), and a grant from the Department of Health and Human Services' Office of Minority Health 1CPIMP091054-04 (BB). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The authors declare no financial disclosures or conflicts of interest.

We posited that religiosity would be associated with lower mortality risk, in part, through AL. Thus, using longitudinal follow-up NHANES III baseline data, we investigated whether religiosity in adults as estimated by church attendance was associated with AL and/or mortality and whether associations with mortality were mediated by AL. We hypothesized that church attendance would be inversely related to AL levels, after adjusting for socioeconomic measures, health insurance status and health behaviors. Given the recent findings of distinct changes in mortality over the last 15 years for middle-aged adults [ 16 ], we focused our primary analysis on persons 40–65 years of age.

Thus, the effect of religiosity or church attendance on health may be mediated in part by a healthy lifestyle, social cohesion or other factors such as mitigating stress. A related potential mediator in this context is allostatic load (AL), a term that refers to the accumulation of physiological perturbations as a result of repeated or chronic stressors in daily life [ 12 – 14 ]. The measurement of AL may include levels of hormones secreted in response to stress and/or related clinical/biologic markers that indirectly reflect the effects of such hormones on the body [ 12 ]. Higher AL has been associated with premature morbidity and mortality in a range of studies and populations [ 15 ], and growing distress was recently reported to underlie the significant rise in the death rate of white middle-aged Americans [ 16 ]. Because religiosity may have a positive impact on stressors, AL levels could vary by level of religiosity.

While interest in the relationship between religion and health is almost as old as humanity, the science relating these rich concepts has grown considerably in the past two decades as increasing numbers of peer-reviewed articles have been reported with results from studies exploring links between religion and various dimensions of physical or mental health [ 1 – 3 ]. Interestingly, the results reported in this body of literature have been mixed. A number of studies have indicated that religion can be beneficial to health [ 3 – 8 ]. However, it is also noteworthy that other studies have found religion to have no effect or, in some cases, a negative association with health outcomes [ 1 , 2 ]. These apparently conflicting findings can be attributed to multiple sources, including differences in the operationalization and measurement of these multidimensional factors. It has been suggested that the beneficial effects of religiosity on the attitudes, motivations, goals, social interactions, and perceptions of individuals about wellness can be assessed by church attendance [ 9 , 10 ]. Prior analyses of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) III linked mortality dataset [1988–1994] have suggested that the association between church attendance and longevity among adult participants may be mediated by other risk factors including health behaviors and inflammation [ 11 ].

Study population characteristics were described by church attendance, using means and standard errors for continuous variables and proportions for categorical variables. All estimates were weighted to adjust for the differential probabilities of sampling and non-response, to represent the total civilian, non-institutionalized US population. Therefore, actual sample sizes are not reported along with the percentages. Estimates derived from a sample size smaller than the recommended lower limit in the NHANES analytic guidelines were considered unreliable [ 26 ]. Logistic regression was used to estimate bivariate AL models and four sequential Cox proportional hazard regressions were constructed to examine the association between the level of church attendance and all-cause mortality. Model 1 included adjustment for race, age, sex, and chronic conditions (asthma, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, non-skin cancer, thyroid disease, rheumatoid arthritis), Model 2 added SES (education, poverty-income ratio, and health insurance status), Model 3 added health behaviors (smoking status, alcohol use, physical activity, and HEI) to Model 2, Model 4 added AL score to Model 3, and Model 5 added self-rated health and social support indicators to Model 4. Results are expressed as hazard ratios (HRs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). All analyses were performed using SAS v 9.4 (Research Triangle Park, NC), and all estimates and statistical tests were adjusted for the complex NHANES survey design.

The church attendance measure was derived from an interview question asking respondents “How often do you attend church or religious services? (per year)”. Responses ranged from not at all to 1095 times a year. Select covariates were included in both sets of models. Behavioral covariates were included physical activity (any vs. none), smoking status (current, former, never), alcohol use (non-drinker, 1–30 drinks/month, >30 drinks per month), and the Healthy Eating Index (HEI) which was scored from 0–100 (higher score represents healthier eating) [ 19 ]. Socioeconomic status (SES) covariates included three variables. Education was categorized as three- variables denoting whether an individual completed <9, 9–12, and >12 years of education. Health insurance was coded as a dichotomous variable indicating whether or not a participant was insured. Income was measured by the poverty-income ratio (PIR), an income-to-needs variable measuring the ratio of household income to the US poverty threshold based on each respondent’s family size and composition at the time of the NHANES III examination [ 19 ]. Self-rated health was assessed with a single item, “How would you rate your overall health?” with categories ranging from excellent to poor [ 22 , 23 ]. In this study, self-rated health was categorized into three groups: “Excellent or Very Good”, “Good”, and “Fair or Poor” [ 24 ]. We also examined social support indicators [ 25 ] as potential covariates using the following questions: a) “In a typical week, how many times do you talk on the telephone with family, friends, or neighbors?”, b) “How often do you get together with friends or relatives; I mean things like going out together or visiting in each other's homes? (per year)”, c) “About how often do you visit with any of your other neighbors, either in their homes or in your own? (per year)”, The natural logarithmic transformation of each social support variable was used in our analyses to compensate for skewness.

Our study population was derived from NHANES III adult participants aged 40–65 years at the time of interview (n = 6,471). Mean duration of follow up for survival was 14.2 years [ 11 ]. Participants who were missing data for two or more components of the AL score (n = 1008), were excluded. Participants with missing data on church attendance were also excluded (n = 14). The final study cohort was comprised of 5,449 participants ( Fig 1 ). A sensitivity analysis was also conducted with all adults aged ≥40 years (final analytic sample = 8,835; supplemental files) and for frequency of church attendance.

Data for our study were drawn from NHANES III (1988–1994), which included approximately 40,000 persons from 89 random US locations [ 17 ]. The NHANES III uses a stratified, multistage probability design to create a nationally representative sample of the civilian, non-institutionalized persons [ 17 ]. The NHANES III also conducts household interviews and collects sociodemographic and clinical information including height, weight and blood pressure, as well as blood samples for specific biochemical analyses [ 18 ]. Because NHANES III data were publicly available and subjects were de-identified, the project was exempt from IRB review.

An NHANES sensitivity analysis using the same strategies for participant ascertainment but without restriction by older age (all participants ≥40 years) revealed similar findings to the main analyses ( S1 – S4 Tables). Odds ratios (OR; 95% CI) for AL scores of 2–3 and ≥ 4 compared to 0–1, by church attendance were OR 1.14 (0.94–1.38), and OR 1.35 (1.11–1.63) respectively. Similar to the primary analysis, a fully adjusted Cox proportional hazard regression model demonstrated a 34% decreased risk 0.66 (0.49–0.87) for all-cause mortality for churchgoers who attended church more than weekly compared to non-churchgoers. Again, statistically significant differences were not noted when we assessed the impact of the frequency of church attendance.

The hazard models for all-cause mortality are presented in Table 4 . Compared to non-churchgoers, churchgoers who attended church more than weekly had a 49% unadjusted reduced risk for all-cause mortality. After adjustment for age, sex, race, and chronic medical conditions churchgoers had a 46% reduction in all-cause mortality. Sequential adjustments for SES (education, health insurance, and PIR; hazard ratio (HR) 0.54), health behaviors (smoking status, physical activity, alcohol use and healthy eating index; (HR) 0.55) minimally affected the HR. Adjustment for stress/cardiovascular indicators (AL) attenuated the HR to 0.60 (0.37–0.95). After controlling for social support indicators and self-rated health to create the fully adjusted model, the HR for churchgoers who attended church more than weekly was 0.45 (0.24–0.85). ( Fig 2 ). For the survival models, we tested and found no significant interaction between church attendance and race or sex, and no effect of AL, stratified by tertiles, on the relationship of church attendance with mortality (data not shown).

In unadjusted analyses, participants who were non-churchgoers had significantly higher rates of high-risk values for 3 of the 10 markers of AL (systolic blood pressure, HDL cholesterol and total cholesterol/ HDL ratio) and higher mean AL scores (p = 0.008) than did those who reported at least monthly church attendance. There was no significant difference in AL based on frequency of church attendance (both at least weekly and at least monthly differed from no church attendance but not from each other, data not shown). Since we did not find a statistically significant dose-response relationship with frequency of church attendance, we compared those who attended some church to non-churchgoers ( Table 3 ). The regression analysis presented in Table 3 revealed that compared to churchgoers, non-churchgoers had a higher AL, with odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) of 1.24 (1.01–1.50) for 2–3 AL components vs. 0–1 and 1.38 (1.11–1.71) for ≥4 components vs. 0–1.

Churchgoers (at least once a year) comprised 64.0% of the study cohort (n = 3,782) and non-churchgoers (no church at all) comprised 36.0% (n = 1,667). The mean (SE) age of the overall cohort was 51±0.2 years and did not differ between churchgoers and non-churchgoers (Tables 1 and 2 ). Baseline characteristics revealed churchgoers had a better socioeconomic and health behavior profile. Specifically, they were more likely to have higher levels of educational attainment, lower levels of poverty, increased physical activity, reduced rates of smoking and drinking, and a healthier eating index. More than 50% of the churchgoers reported they had “Excellent/Very Good Health”. Overall, non-churchgoers spent more time seeking social support from family, friends, relatives, and neighbors than churchgoers. ( Table 1 ).

This study examined the association of church attendance with prevalent AL and all-cause mortality in a large multiethnic sample. We found that the lack of church attendance (a surrogate for religiosity) was associated with a greater likelihood of having an elevated AL (a measure of physiologic dysregulation) and all-cause mortality that persisted after further adjustment of socio-demographic, clinical and laboratory confounders. The higher adjusted mortality rate for non-churchgoers was slightly attenuated after controlling for AL, but remained statistically significant, suggesting the potential influence of religiosity independent of mediation by AL.

Our findings are consistent with prior studies that linked AL with mortality [19, 27]. They also build upon, but are distinct from, the work of Gillum et al. who found a lower unadjusted mortality rate among weekly and more-than-weekly church attendees compared with non-attendees using NHANES III participants ages 40 and over; but this association was no longer significant after controlling for common health-related mediating factors [11]. In contrast to the analysis by Gillum and colleagues, our primary analyses focus on premature mortality among participants aged 40–65, who represent a particularly vulnerable group, and we examined AL as a potential mediator of the relationship between church attendance and mortality. We also performed a sensitivity analysis of mortality in persons ≥ 40 years of age to examine whether the longer follow up in our analyses–14.2 years in contrast to 8.5 years in the Gillum paper—resulted in a different association between church attendance and mortality. Our findings are consistent with a possible independent effect of church attendance on mortality that is consistent with data recently reported by Li et al. [28] in over 74,000 women in the Nurses’ Health Study. They also indicate that church attendance and possibly religiosity may mitigate the effect of stressors on physiologic dysregulation.

Our findings support the overall hypothesis that increased religiosity (as determined by church attendance) is associated with less stress (as assessed by AL) and enhanced longevity. The positive relationship of church attendance with both reduced AL and longevity suggest religiosity can affect two well-described objective health parameters. The role of church attendance in enhancing longevity may be mediated through several causal pathways linking religion and physical health such as psychological traits, lifestyle decisions, social conditions and social networks as described by Koenig [29], Li et al. [28] and Hummer et al. [22]. Thus, we also controlled for self reported health which has been reported to be a powerful indicator of mortality [23] and time spent seeking social support from family, friends, relatives, and neighbors [22] in an effort to address additional key elements not incorporated by allostatic load, our physiologic measure of chronic stress. Even with the adjustments the association of church attendance with reduced mortality remained constant.

Health benefits of religiosity may also be attributed, in part, to its impact on two less commonly cited domains, compassion [30] and a sense of holiness [31]. Compassion has a long history of association with religiosity [32] and has been reported to mediate some of the psychosocial health benefits of religiosity and social relationships through its related generosity and altruism [33], as well as reducing stress and/or enhancing coping skills [34]. Holiness gives meaning and purpose to life [35] and inspires commitment to something greater than self [36]. Holiness instills love, joy, peace, hope and fulfillment [37], fosters a sense of inter-connectedness with others [38], promotes a sense of wholeness in life [39], and engenders a greater personal relationship with a higher power [40], possibly explaining the health benefits attributed to religiosity [41, 42].

Although these analyses support the literature linking religiosity and mortality, we found no evidence that the actual frequency of church attendance offered similar protection as no differences in AL or mortality were found between those attending church at least weekly and those attending church at least monthly. Allostatic load has been described as a multifactorial concept that links an individual with their local environmental stressors and the balance of different components may eventually manifest as a distinct disease(s). Specifically, individual gene polymorphisms juxtaposed with life experiences, environmental stressors and either adaptive behavioral responses (e.g. exercise, good nutrition) or maladaptive behavioral responses (e.g. substance abuse) influence the development and progression of many disease states [12]. An increased AL and associated organ dysfunction may result from increased frequency or degree of select environmental stressors, ability or inability to adapt to such stressors over time, and the psychological impact of stress such as anticipation (e.g., worrying about a possible stressful event in the future that may or may not take place) and/or memories of prior stressors (e.g., post-traumatic stress disorder) [12]. Similarly, Cohen and colleagues reported that environment stress can result in maladaptive coping mechanisms such as negative emotional states, superimposed psychological distress and/or the adoption of unhealthy health beliefs and behaviors [43]. The confluence of these psychological and behavioral responses may lead to long-term physiological changes that are characterized by increased AL and ultimately organ dysfunction. Our findings that the compilation of stress responses assessed by AL is more common among non-church attendees than church attendees are supportive of these concepts and consistent with Powell et al. who suggested religiosity/spirituality is strongly linked to improved health outcomes in the general population [44]. Health outcomes may also differ across different religious groups and practices. For example, Seventh-day Adventists have been reported to experience lower risks of diabetes mellitus, hypertension, and arthritis. It has been suggested these patterns are likely due to the Adventists’ predominantly vegetarian diet; however, other more religious-based mechanisms cannot be excluded [45].

Our study had several limitations. The clinical/biologic markers assessed, while commonly studied, are a substantive but not a complete measure of physiological dysregulation, and are unlikely to fully capture alterations in immune function, inflammatory responses, or neuroendocrine function, such as that regulated by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. The NHANES III dataset does not include lab values on markers of the HPA axis such as cortisol and epinephrine. It is likely that due to lack of data on duration and/or severity of components of the overall AL measure our crude summary score may not be optimal. While we adjusted for many biologic and environmental factors, we cannot exclude residual confounding and since this is an observational study, one cannot infer causality. It is also possible that the health benefits of religiosity have been underestimated because of the crude or imprecise measurement. Church attendance can represent a public expression of religiosity but does not account for private practices such as prayer. Lastly, while we have postulated that church attendance in this survey may reflect religiosity, and we attempted to adjust for the potential positive impact of church attendance on socialization or social biases against those not attending church [22], we cannot exclude residual confounding from factors such as disease severity/duration or other aspects of social support not included in our model. Each of these might affect the observed differences in the associations for both stress and mortality among church attendees compared to non-church attendees.

In conclusion, we found a significant relationship between church attendance and mortality in middle-aged (≥40–65 yrs) adults NHANES III participants with an extended mean follow up time of 14 years. This relationship remained significant even after adjustment for education, poverty status, health insurance status, self-rated health, social support, and AL, suggesting a potential independent effect of religiosity on mortality. Similar findings were also noted in a secondary analysis of NHANES III participants ≥ 40 years old. Our results underscore the potential importance of church attendance as a surrogate for religiosity as a mediator of health and lifespan. The increased attention to religiosity and other faith-related factors by health professionals and scientists is warranted by these findings and those from similar studies [46]. Results from this study contribute to the existing body of evidence and support the need for more rigorous prospective studies to explore causal relationships of religiosity and health.

twunda on June 18th, 2017 at 11:53 UTC »

Here's a 2009 study with similar results.

Edit:

A 2005 study

A 2011 Study

The general theory seems to be that those who attend services regularly have the combined benefits of supportive communities and heathier behaviors (e.g.: not smoking).

SummerMummer on June 18th, 2017 at 11:25 UTC »

How does that compare to people who often get out of the house for other types of social interaction (volunteering, etc.)?

wwabc on June 18th, 2017 at 11:22 UTC »

"The church attendance measure was derived from an interview question asking respondents “How often do you attend church or religious services? (per year)”. Responses ranged from not at all to 1095 times a year."

some people go three times a day? wow

is this just comparing active people to non-active people? If someone went to watch a sporting event more than once a week, would the results be similar?