“Companion dog acquisition” has a scientifically proven benefit

Authored by inverse.com and submitted by mvea

Feeling lonely? A dog may help. Our research confirms what many dog owners already know: dogs are great companions that can help you to feel less lonely.

Cuddles and slobbery kisses, meeting other dog owners in the park, and a general lift in mood all likely help.

But our study, published Tuesday in BMC Public Health, found dogs didn’t affect psychological distress, the type seen in depression and anxiety.

Almost two in five Australian households own a dog. And although most dog owners will assure you, in no uncertain terms, their dog is a source of sheer happiness, scientific evidence is lacking.

Most previous studies have compared the mental well-being of dog owners to non-owners at a single point in time. The problem with these studies is they cannot tell if dogs actually make us happier, less lonely, or less stressed. They also cannot tell us if dog owners are simply in a more positive state of mind in the first place.

So, in this study, we measured mental well-being at three points in time: before owning a dog, three months after owning a dog, and eight months after owning a dog.

Our study, known as the PAWS trial, involved 71 Sydney adults who were separated into three groups:

People who bought a dog within one month of starting the study.

People who were interested in getting a dog in the near future but agreed not to get one during the study.

People who had no interest in getting a dog.

People filled out surveys to measure their mood, loneliness, and symptoms of psychological distress at the three different time points. We then compared the mental well-being of the groups at the beginning of the study, to the mid-point, and to the end-point.

New dog owners felt less lonely after they got a dog compared to the other two groups. The effect happened quite quickly, within three months of acquiring a dog. There was no further decrease in loneliness between three months and eight months.

The joy of a new dog eased loneliness within the first few months.

We also found some evidence that dog owners had fewer negative emotions, such as nervousness or distress, within three months of getting a new dog, but this finding was not as clear-cut.

We found that symptoms of depression and anxiety were unchanged after acquiring a dog. Maybe the dog owners in our study already had low levels of psychological distress before they got a dog, so dog ownership didn’t lower these levels any further.

There are lots of possible reasons dogs can help to lessen feelings of loneliness. We know having a quick cuddle with a dog boosts people’s mood in the short-term. Maybe daily dog cuddles can also boost owners’ mood in the long-term, which could help to lower feelings of loneliness.

Dog owners may also meet new people through their dog, as people are more likely to talk strangers if they are accompanied by a dog. In our study, dog owners also said they had met new people in their neighborhood because of their dog.

So far, there have only been two similar studies to look at mental well-being in new dog owners, one of which was conducted almost 30 years ago.

Of these studies, one found dog owners had fewer symptoms of psychiatric disorders after they acquired a dog. The other study found no difference in loneliness after people brought a new dog home.

Dogs may also improve our physical health, by reducing blood pressure, improving cardiovascular health, and increasing the amount of physical activity their owners perform. But, as is the case with mental well-being, the scientific evidence is still limited.

One of the things our study cannot determine is how dogs affect men’s mental well-being. By chance, all the new dog owners in our study were women. So, we don’t know whether dogs affect men’s mental well-being in a different way to women’s.

Our next step is to look at mental well-being in a much bigger group of new dog owners to confirm these findings. A bigger study could also provide more insight into the relationship between dog ownership and mental illness, such as depression and anxiety.

If this article has raised issues for you, or if you’re concerned about someone you know, call Lifeline on 13 11 14.

This article was originally published on The Conversation by Lauren Powell and Emmanuel Stamatakis. Read the original article here.

The420Conspiracy on November 10th, 2019 at 16:12 UTC »

Yes but dogs require alot of work an attention please remember this and don’t just buy a dog because you are depressed.

DonManuel on November 10th, 2019 at 11:40 UTC »

Prison dogs are even capable of resocialization of really hardcore criminals.

mvea on November 10th, 2019 at 10:36 UTC »

The post title is a copy and paste from the first, twelfth and thirteenth paragraphs of the linked popular press article here:

Our research confirms what many dog owners already know: dogs are great companions that can help you to feel less lonely.

New dog owners felt less lonely after they got a dog compared to the other two groups. The effect happened quite quickly, within three months of acquiring a dog.

We also found some evidence that dog owners had fewer negative emotions, such as nervousness or distress, within three months of getting a new dog, but this finding was not as clear-cut.

Journal Reference:

Powell, L., Edwards, K.M., McGreevy, P. et al.

Companion dog acquisition and mental well-being: a community-based three-arm controlled study.

BMC Public Health 19, 1428 (2019)

Link: https://bmcpublichealth.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12889-019-7770-5

doi:10.1186/s12889-019-7770-5

Abstract

Background

Dog ownership is suggested to improve mental well-being, although empirical evidence among community dog owners is limited. This study examined changes in human mental well-being following dog acquisition, including four measures: loneliness, positive and negative affect, and psychological distress.

Methods

We conducted an eight-month controlled study involving three groups (n = 71): 17 acquired a dog within 1 month of baseline (dog acquisition); 29 delayed dog acquisition until study completion (lagged control); and 25 had no intentions of acquiring a dog (community control). All participants completed the UCLA Loneliness Scale (possible scores 0–60), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule and Kessler10 at baseline, three-months and eight-months. We used repeated measures ANCOVAs to analyse data with owner age and sex included as covariates. Post-hoc tests were performed for significant effects (p < 0.05).

Results

There was a statistically significant group by time interaction for loneliness (p = 0.03), with an estimated reduction of 8.41 units (95% CI -16.57, − 0.26) from baseline to three-months and 7.12 (95% CI -12.55, − 1.69) from baseline to eight-months in the dog acquisition group. The group by time interaction for positive affect was also significant (p = 0.03), although there was no change in the dog acquisition group.

Conclusions

Companion dog acquisition may reduce loneliness among community dog owners. Our study provides useful direction for future larger trials on the effects of dog ownership on human mental well-being.