Metabolic costs and evolutionary implications of human brain development

Authored by ncbi.nlm.nih.gov and submitted by eric2332
image for Metabolic costs and evolutionary implications of human brain development

The metabolic costs of brain development are thought to explain the evolution of humans’ exceptionally slow and protracted childhood growth; however, the costs of the human brain during development are unknown. We used existing PET and MRI data to calculate brain glucose use from birth to adulthood. We find that the brain’s metabolic requirements peak in childhood, when it uses glucose at a rate equivalent to 66% of the body’s resting metabolism and 43% of the body’s daily energy requirement, and that brain glucose demand relates inversely to body growth from infancy to puberty. Our findings support the hypothesis that the unusually high costs of human brain development require a compensatory slowing of childhood body growth.

The high energetic costs of human brain development have been hypothesized to explain distinctive human traits, including exceptionally slow and protracted preadult growth. Although widely assumed to constrain life-history evolution, the metabolic requirements of the growing human brain are unknown. We combined previously collected PET and MRI data to calculate the human brain’s glucose use from birth to adulthood, which we compare with body growth rate. We evaluate the strength of brain–body metabolic trade-offs using the ratios of brain glucose uptake to the body’s resting metabolic rate (RMR) and daily energy requirements (DER) expressed in glucose-gram equivalents (glucose rmr% and glucose der% ). We find that glucose rmr% and glucose der% do not peak at birth (52.5% and 59.8% of RMR, or 35.4% and 38.7% of DER, for males and females, respectively), when relative brain size is largest, but rather in childhood (66.3% and 65.0% of RMR and 43.3% and 43.8% of DER). Body-weight growth (dw/dt) and both glucose rmr% and glucose der% are strongly, inversely related: soon after birth, increases in brain glucose demand are accompanied by proportionate decreases in dw/dt. Ages of peak brain glucose demand and lowest dw/dt co-occur and subsequent developmental declines in brain metabolism are matched by proportionate increases in dw/dt until puberty. The finding that human brain glucose demands peak during childhood, and evidence that brain metabolism and body growth rate covary inversely across development, support the hypothesis that the high costs of human brain development require compensatory slowing of body growth rate.

To quantify the metabolic costs of the human brain, in this study we used a unique, previously collected age series of PET measures of brain glucose uptake spanning birth to adulthood ( 32 ), along with existing MRI volumetric data ( 36 ), to calculate the brain’s total glucose use from birth to adulthood, which we compare with body growth rate. We estimate total brain glucose uptake by age (inclusive of all oxidative and nonoxidative functions), which we compare with two measures of whole-body energy expenditure: RMR, reflecting maintenance functions only, and daily energy requirements (DER), reflecting the combination of maintenance, activity, and growth. We hypothesized that ages of peak substrate competition (i.e., competition for glucose) between brain and body would be aligned developmentally with the age of slowest childhood body growth, and more generally that growth rate and brain glucose use would covary inversely during development, as is predicted by the concept of a trade-off between brain metabolism and body growth in human life-history evolution.

In contrast with the assumption that the per-gram brain metabolic rate is stable with age, PET studies show that glucose uptake in the cerebral cortex is more than twice as high during early- to midchildhood than in adulthood ( 32 ). This dynamism reflects the additional energetic costs associated with overproliferation of neuronal processes and synapses before activity-dependent pruning in late childhood and adolescence ( 33 , 34 ), along with aerobic glycolysis, which is thought to rise in support of synaptic growth ( 27 – 29 ). In contrast, at birth, before extensive postnatal synaptic proliferation and the corresponding rise in aerobic glycolysis, PET-derived glucose uptake is 20–30% lower than in adults ( 32 ). Although past PET studies provide insights into the dynamics of glucose use at various stages of development, these studies only report point estimates in specific brain structures ( 32 , 35 ), thus leaving the costs of the entire human brain during development—and potential evolutionary trade-offs with body growth—uncertain.

Although past work points to high metabolic requirements of the growing human brain, data are available for only a small number of individuals of restricted age range, which limits their utility for evaluating trade-offs between brain metabolism and body growth during development. The only previous attempt to derive a growth curve for brain metabolism that we are aware of assumed that the mass-specific metabolic rate of the brain, measured using the N 2 O method ( 24 ), is stable at adult-like levels across development ( 25 , 31 ). Using this method, it was estimated that the brain accounts for 87% of RMR at birth, and that this fraction then steadily declines as the brain-to-body mass ratio decreases with age ( 25 ). This finding, if correct, would not support the hypothesis that ages of slowest body growth, in childhood, coincide with ages of peak brain metabolic requirements.

Although the energetic trade-off concept is widely cited, the ability to test it against alternative hypotheses ( 2 , 5 , 10 , 11 ) has been hampered by limitations of prior measures of brain metabolism during human growth. Direct estimates of total brain energy use have been based upon the nitrous oxide (N 2 O) method, which quantifies brain oxygen consumption and estimates energy expenditure based on an assumption that glucose exclusively enters oxidative phosphorylation ( 24 ). However, recent work shows that the rate of glucose uptake exceeds oxygen consumption in the brain ( 27 ), with up to 30% of brain glucose not entering oxidative phosphorylation during childhood ( 28 ). This additional use of glucose, in aerobic glycolysis, contributes to protein synthesis associated with synaptic growth and other important developmental functions ( 28 – 30 ), yet is not reflected in measures of oxygen consumption like N 2 O, which therefore underestimate total brain glucose uptake and use.

Other hypotheses have focused on energetic trade-offs and viewed slowed growth as compensation for the brain’s high energetic needs ( 1 , 14 – 19 ). Because of its large size, the human brain has unusually high energy costs ( 15 , 20 , 21 ), which are particularly elevated compared with the body’s metabolic budget early in the life cycle ( 18 , 22 ). It has been estimated that the human brain accounts for between 44% and 87% of resting metabolic rate (RMR) during infancy, childhood, and adolescence ( 23 – 25 ), suggesting strong trade-offs with other functions. The human brain’s demand for energy is sufficiently high during these periods that it could require that the body expend less on growth, thus slowing and prolonging the preadult period ( 1 , 14 – 18 , 26 ). This reasoning leads to the expectation that the ages of slowest body growth will coincide developmentally with peak brain metabolic needs.

Many hypotheses have been proposed to explain this slow and prolonged preadult life-stage, with most pointing to the extra time and energy required for human learning and brain development ( 5 , 10 – 12 ). It has long been assumed that human cultural practices are sufficiently complex that they take many years to learn, which could have selected for a slowing down and extension of preadult development ( 13 ). A recent variant of this idea notes the importance of complex extractive techniques to human foraging success, and proposes that humans have a long preadult stage to facilitate mastery of these skills ( 2 ).

A prolonged period of childhood and juvenile growth is a defining feature of human life history ( 1 – 3 ). Compared with other great apes, human offspring are weaned early, leading to an extended period of dependence on procured resources rather than breast milk ( 1 , 4 ). Although this unique human reproductive pattern is viewed as shortening the interbirth interval and thus increasing fertility ( 5 , 6 ), what is less clear is why humans also grow so slowly during childhood. Although most primates grow slower than other mammals ( 7 ), human childhood and juvenile growth stand out as unusually slow even by primate and great ape standards, during which it proceeds at a pace more typical of reptiles than of mammals ( 8 , 9 ). In humans, a sizeable percentage of preadult growth is deferred until the pubertal growth spurt, when growth rate markedly increases and adult size is achieved ( 1 ).

Finally, we compared age changes in glucose rmr% and glucose der% with body-weight growth velocity, calculated as the derivative (dw/dt) of the function linking body weight with age. Plotting unitless SD scores of glucose rmr% and dw/dt against age reveals opposing age trends in the two measures starting at 6 mo of age ( ). When SD scores of dw/dt are plotted against those of glucose rmr% ( ), the first 6 mo are characterized by a fast but decelerating rate of body-weight growth and a parallel reduction in glucose rmr% . Starting at 6 mo, each increase in glucose rmr% is matched by a proportionate decrease in dw/dt, with peak glucose rmr% and the trough of dw/dt co-occurring. After the childhood peak in brain glucose demands, each subsequent age-related decline in glucose rmr% is accompanied by a proportionate increase in dw/dt until onset of pubertal weight gain (∼12–13 y in males and ∼9–10 y in females), after which slower growth is accompanied by declining glucose rmr% as both body size and brain metabolism approach adult levels. Very similar results were obtained when brain glucose was evaluated in relation to DER ( ). For reference, predicted values of key variables are reported for males and females in SI Appendix, Tables S1 and S2 , respectively.

To evaluate developmental changes in the strength of substrate competition between the brain and body, we next calculated the body’s RMR and DER by entering the body weights ( SI Appendix, Fig. S3 ) of the individuals for whom brain weights were obtained ( 37 ) into age- and sex-specific predictive equations ( 38 ). To allow direct comparison with brain glucose uptake, RMR ( SI Appendix, Fig. S4 ) and DER ( SI Appendix, Fig. S5 ) were first converted to grams of glucose equivalents (1 g glucose = 3.72 kcal of energy expenditure) ( 39 ). We expressed metabolic competition between brain and body as glucose rmr% and glucose der% , calculated as the ratios between brain glucose uptake and daily glucose-gram equivalent RMR or DER, respectively. These ratios do not indicate the percent of RMR or DER used by the brain because some brain glucose uptake (the numerator) is not used in energy production ( 28 ). Instead, glucose rmr% and glucose der% can be interpreted as the fraction of the body’s RMR or DER that could be met by the quantity of glucose consumed by the brain (see SI Appendix, SI Materials and Methods for details). In males and females, respectively, total brain glucose uptake is the equivalent of 52.5% and 59.8% of RMR at birth, drops to 37.5 and 40.8% of RMR in the first half-year, then rises to a lifetime peak of 66.3% and 65.0% of RMR by 4.2–4.4 y ( and SI Appendix, Fig. S6 ). Although the brain accounted for a smaller fraction of DER, the pattern of glucose consumption relative to DER was very similar to that for RMR ( ): glucose der% accounted for the equivalent of 35.4% and 38.7% of DER at birth, declined to 24.7% and 26.8% by 7 mo, before rising to peak levels of 43.3% at 3.8 y (males) and 43.8% at 4 y (females). Adult glucose rmr% was 19.1% and 24.0% whereas glucose der% was 10.9% and 15.0% in males and females, respectively.

We first fit continuous functions to point estimates, inclusive of gray and white matter, of glucose uptake for the cerebrum, brainstem, and cerebellum in 29 individuals ( 32 ) ranging in age from birth to late adolescence ( SI Appendix, Fig. S1 ). Regional glucose uptake rates were then multiplied by regional brain weights to obtain the daily grams of glucose consumed by the entire brain in both oxidative metabolism and aerobic glycolysis (for details, see Materials and Methods ). Daily glucose use by the brain peaks at 5.2 y of age at 167.0 g/d and 146.1 g/d in males and females, respectively. These values represent 1.88- and 1.82-times the daily glucose use of the brain in adulthood ( and SI Appendix, Fig. S2 ), despite the fact that body size is more than three-times as large in the adult.

Our findings agree with past estimates indicating that the brain dominates the body’s metabolism during early life (31). However, our PET-based calculations reveal that the magnitude of brain glucose uptake, both in absolute terms and relative to the body’s metabolic budget, does not peak at birth but rather in childhood, when the glucose used by the brain comprises the equivalent of 66% of the body’s RMR, and roughly 43% of total expenditure. These findings are in broad agreement with past clinical work showing that the body’s mass-specific glucose production rates are highest in childhood, and tightly linked with the brain’s metabolic needs (40). Whereas past attempts to quantify the contribution of the brain to the body’s metabolic expenditure suggested that the brain accounted for a continuously decreasing fraction of RMR as the brain-to-body weight ratio declined with age (25, 31), we find a more complex pattern of substrate trade-off. Both glucose rmr% and glucose der% decline in the first half-year as a fast but decelerating pace of body growth established in utero initially outpaces postnatal increases in brain metabolism. Beginning around 6 mo, increases in relative glucose use are matched by proportionate decreases in weight growth, whereas ages of declining brain glucose uptake in late childhood and early adolescence are accompanied by proportionate increases in weight growth. The relationships that we document between age changes in brain glucose demands and body-weight growth rate are particularly striking in males, who maintain these inverse linear trends despite experiencing threefold changes in brain glucose demand and body growth rate between 6 mo and 13 y of age. In females, an earlier onset of pubertal weight gain leads to earlier deviations from similar linear inverse relationships.

Our results shed light on several unique features of human life-history evolution. The long period of slow human childhood and juvenile growth has alternately been viewed as, among other hypotheses, necessary for learning of complex foraging skills (2), as a by-product of selection for lifespan extension (5), as allowing greater activity devoted to subsistence tasks that contribute to the family’s pooled energy budget (10), as reducing the dietary burden on human mothers raising multiple overlapping dependent offspring (11), or as compensation for high brain energy needs (1, 14–18). Our finding of a strong, inverse relationship between developmental changes in the brain’s glucose uptake and body-weight growth rate supports the last hypothesis. It is notable that the increase in body growth rate at puberty—which is unusually pronounced in humans—is deferred to an age when brain glucose uptake is greatly reduced (1, 26, 41). We interpret our finding of an inverse relationship between the brain’s demand for glucose and body-weight growth rate as support for the hypothesis that human brain development is sufficiently costly to require a compensatory reduction in expenditure on body growth, thus helping explain our unusually slow rate of childhood growth and resultant delay in attainment of adult size and sexual maturity compared with other closely related great apes.

The childhood peak in brain glucose uptake is greatest in the cerebrum, and less pronounced in the brainstem and cerebellum (32). These patterns suggest that peak brain glucose uptake during childhood reflects neuronal plasticity in the cerebral cortex (32), which involves overproliferation of energetically costly dendritic arbors and synapses before activity-dependent pruning (33, 34). Recent work shows that glucose uptake outpaces oxygen use in the human brain, and that this imbalance also peaks at the age of greatest glucose uptake in childhood, pointing to an important role of aerobic glycolysis in support of synaptic proliferation and growth (28, 30). Collectively, this large increase in glucose uptake corresponds closely with the age of slowest body-weight growth.

Although we find that body growth rate and brain substrate use covary inversely during human development, pointing to a likely trade-off between these functions, our findings lead to the more general prediction that other costly somatic or physiological expenditures will also be reduced at this age to free up energy and substrate to support brain development. Consistent with this perspective, recent studies of total energy expenditure (TEE) show that typical physical activity levels (PAL = TEE/RMR) for preschool age children (3–5 y) are lower than in later childhood and adolescence (38, 39, 42). These findings suggest that, like the body’s very low expenditure on somatic growth, activity-related (discretionary) expenditure during human development is also comparatively low at ages of peak brain metabolic demand.

Pontzer et al. (43) recently showed that humans and other primates have reduced TEEs for their body sizes, despite having basal metabolic rates (BMRs) consistent with those of other mammals. The authors conclude that a reduced rate of energy throughput helps explain some shared features of primate life histories, including slower growth compared with other mammals. Pontzer et al. speculate that high brain energy needs might account for the fact that primate BMRs are not similarly reduced, but they also note extensive life-history variation among primates that traces to differences in allocation priorities. Our findings complement these data and implicate the high energy and substrate requirements of the brain, especially during the ages of most rapid development and learning, as a likely cause of life-history differences between humans and other closely related primates, such as chimpanzees.

Because the costs of neocortical synaptic growth and metabolism comprise a major endpoint for glucose in the brain, examination of the generalizability of the brain energetics–body growth trade-off hypothesis can be informed by data on postnatal synaptogenesis in other primates, which are available for humans, chimpanzees, and macaques. In all three species neocortical synapse densities are doubled during growth, compared with densities at birth. In macaques the period of peak synaptic density occurs during infancy, whereas in humans and chimpanzees peak synaptic densities take place during the midjuvenile or (in humans) childhood period (44). Brain energetics are thus not likely to impose a strong constraint on later juvenile body growth in macaques, whereas in chimpanzees growth is predicted to be slowed in the juvenile period, but to a lesser degree than in humans because of their smaller brains and corresponding lower brain metabolic needs (22). Comparative growth data support this prediction (41), showing that, unlike humans, chimpanzees and other great apes do not display marked declines in weight velocities after infancy. Additionally, humans are not distinct in having a “growth spurt” but in delaying it until after a long period of slow childhood growth (41).

Our findings also lead to the prediction that a slowing of preadult growth and increase in brain metabolism coevolved in the course of human evolution. Estimates of body growth from fragmentary and limited fossil hominin remains can be complicated; nonetheless, current data on hominin dental emergence, enamel growth rate, and skeletal growth suggest that a more extended period between weaning and sexual maturity began to appear at least 1.5 million y ago in Homo erectus (45). Data on dental eruption point to coevolution between brain expansion and maturational delay (45, 46), with a fully modern pattern of delayed physical growth not emerging until the origin of anatomically modern humans (47, 48). Although these findings are generally consistent with the hypothesis that brain expansion has set the pace for changes in body-size growth in the hominin lineage, our results underscore the challenges of interpreting the strength of metabolic trade-offs related to human brain evolution from data on the cranial capacity of fossils alone (e.g., ref. 14). In the modern human sample evaluated here, an average gram of brain tissue at 4 y uses more than 2.5-times the glucose of a gram of brain at birth (see data in SI Appendix, Tables S1 and S2). Absolute brain glucose requirements peak at ∼5 y, several years before final brain size is achieved, and adult brain glucose requirements are only half those at age 5 y. This uncoupling of the energetic requirements of the brain from brain size reflects developmental dynamics in substrate-intensive processes related to neural plasticity and learning, which are not preserved in fossil samples. In this context, it is notable that, despite having similar cranial capacities, dental eruption data suggest that Neanderthals grew more rapidly than modern humans (48).

Although it is not possible to precisely evaluate the coevolution between brain metabolic requirements and slowing of body growth from the fossil record, the ontogeny of brain metabolism that we document provides clues into the ecological strategies that were likely required for the modern human pattern of brain development to evolve. It has been noted that the high and inflexible glucose requirements of the developing human brain make it susceptible to impairment from nutritional shortfall (18). Despite this finding, we find that peak brain glucose uptake occurs after the age of complete weaning from breast milk in most human societies (4), and when the energetic buffer of body fat stores are near their lifetime nadir (18). In light of this finding, it seems likely that the evolution of human encephalization required cultural or social strategies to buffer the energy intake of dependent offspring. Rudimentary hunting and gathering economies appeared with the emergence of the genus Homo and are believed to have been important in supporting early brain expansion (49–51). A shift to calorically dense and easily digested foods, and greater food sharing among social groups, would have increased the nutritional quality and stability of the diet (14, 15, 20). Although direct evidence for childcare strategies are not preserved in the fossil record, recent comparative analyses suggest that cooperative care in raising and provisioning young was likely important in achieving levels of encephalization seen in Homo sapiens (52, 53). A system in which extended social networks provisioned food for children, combined with shifts to calorically dense and easily digested foods procured through hunting (14, 15), would have allowed the costs of human brain development to be widely distributed and also buffered against shortfall. This shift in the social context of human child rearing, occurring together with a protracted postnatal development of cortical connectivity (54, 55), likely created new opportunities for imitative learning and cumulative, intergenerational development of cultural traditions to become central features of the human adaptive complex (56, 57).

Our results also highlight underappreciated implications of the high glucose requirements of the human brain for medicine and public health. Diabetes is a condition in which tissues (primarily muscle) fail to respond appropriately to insulin, thus reducing glucose uptake. Although an important precursor to type 2 diabetes, peripheral insulin resistance is also effective at shunting scarce glucose to high-demand tissues or organs, such as to the fetus during the mild diabetes of pregnancy (58), or to the brain during the insulin-resistance triggered by stress (59). Changes in insulin secretion are believed to help coordinate glucose delivery to the brain as brain size and energy requirements change during development (60, 61). The present finding that the brain’s requirement for glucose peaks after weaning, at an age when body fat stores are minimal (18), points to strong selection on physiologic mechanisms to redirect glucose delivery to the brain during nutritional stress. Studies report interactions between fetal undernutrition or small birth size and childhood weight gain as predictors of diabetes risk, suggesting an important role for childhood energetics and substrate availability in diabetes development (62, 63). Although normative data on developmental changes in insulin sensitivity during child development are lacking (64), our findings lead to the prediction that facultative buffering of substrate supply to the brain (e.g., insulin resistance induced acutely by stress) will be prominent at 4–5 y of age (59).

Measuring brain metabolism in living children poses unique challenges that warrant discussion. For ethical reasons, there is no study that includes PET and MRI data in the same healthy individuals spanning birth to adulthood. Consequently, we used brain volumetrics and glucose uptake rates obtained from two different United States samples deemed developmentally normal before screening (MRI) or after clinical assessments for possible abnormalities turned up negative (PET) (see Materials and Methods for more details). An additional limitation is that our analyses are based on cross-sectional brain and body-weight data, which pool fast- and slow-maturing individuals; this pooling can attenuate the apparent magnitude of the pubertal growth spurt while also obscuring the variable timings of its onset (65). Analytic techniques that account for variation in maturational tempo, such as centering of individual growth curves on maturational indices rather than chronological age (65), would require repeat PET imaging in healthy individuals, which, as noted, is not ethical. The age of the trough in body-weight growth rate estimated from the cross-sectional data used here is consistent with mixed-longitudinal reference data showing lowest rates of weight gain from 4 to 4.5 y (66), and the latest World Health Organization (WHO) report on human energy requirements similarly finds that caloric needs for body growth are lowest at 4–5 y (38). Importantly, the cross-sectional body weights that we use to calculate RMR and the brain masses used to calculate brain glucose uptake were obtained from the same individuals, suggesting that our approach provides a useful basis for evaluating the changing relationship between brain metabolism and body size across a large sample varying widely in age and developmental stage.

In sum, we find that the rate of glucose uptake by the human brain, in both absolute terms and relative to the body’s metabolic expenditure, does not peak at birth, when the size of the brain relative to the body is largest, but in childhood, when synaptic densities and related metabolic processes are maximal. These new estimates show that the ages when substrate trade-offs between the brain and body are greatest in humans coincide developmentally with the ages of slowest childhood body-weight growth, and that age-related changes in relative brain glucose needs and body-weight gain are tightly, inversely related from early infancy until puberty. These findings thus provide rare empirical support for the hypothesis that humans evolved a protracted period of slow preadult growth to compensate for the unusually high metabolic costs of brain development underlying the extraordinary human capacity for cultural learning.

thehollowman84 on August 27th, 2019 at 13:00 UTC »

In resting adults it's 20% of the energy. The brain is the most complex thing in the universe (that we know of). It takes 18+ years for it to mature.

drown_the_rabbit on August 27th, 2019 at 12:34 UTC »

Adults use about 20% of their energy to operate their brain. A newborn baby has so many neural connections being developed in a short span, this makes complete sense

Lil-Maece on August 27th, 2019 at 11:07 UTC »

5% energy for lava pooping, 5% for spontaneous projectile vomiting, 3% for everything else.